READING
GUIDE
TO:
Sociology Research Group in Cultural and
Education Studies (eds) (1980) Melbourne Working
Papers 1980, University of Melbourne
by Dave Harris
[This collection contains two
pieces by Bourdieu et al that seem to be the raw
conference paper form of two of the chapters in Academic Discourse.Both of these papers
contain much more detail, however, including
detail of the empirical work undertaken on student
language which is referred to in the published
volume]
Bourdieu and Passeron Introduction:
language and pedagogical situation
The words
in lectures are used to ‘dazzle rather than
enlighten’ (38), maintaining a respectful distance
between students and lecturers.The claims are that this is simply an
efficient form of transmission [enhanced by the
paraphernalia of outcomes and so on], but there is
a massive ‘loss of information’ seen in student
work.The
inefficiencies need to be discussed.Misunderstanding is both a social and a
political function of university discourse.University communication
is an element in the whole system, referring to
the relations of institutional and material
conditions.It
guarantees both frustration and security.
It is possible to test the
efficiency of academic language, for example to
test the understanding of the vocabulary used in
lectures.Students
are often simply resigned to use these mystifying
terms, as their essays often show.Essays develop a ‘ rhetoric of despair’,
which students use to reassure themselves, and
they also indicate Creole versions of academic
language, which are ‘most characteristic of
magisterial language’ (40).
It is not just the use of
jargon which baffles students.Even sociology and philosophy students form
this Creole version, even though they are trained
in the specific uses of words and syntactic rigour
[however, philosophy lectures are criticised as
being based on slogans, leaving too many terms
undefined, confusing ‘concrete vocabulary’ with ‘abstract vocabulary’,
operating with an notion of slow apprenticeship
rather than explicit communication or mapping, and
refusing to demonstrate specific difficulties
(41)].Academic code
is the issue.The
problem is often misunderstood as one of quantity,
but really, the code can only be learned ‘through
the decoding of less and less unskilled messages’,
a kind of apprenticeship, a matter of ‘diffuse
socialization’ (41).Properly
pedagogical
communication should transmit the code and reduce
unnecessary noise.However,
students are prepared to see noise as a necessary
evil, and not to apply the usual demand of maximum
information for minimum cost.The demand for pedagogical communication
actually is pedagogical itself.Its efficiency can be tested by estimating
the percentage of transmitted information which is
actually received [both ways—from lectures to
student notes, and from student work back to
lecturers]. We need to take the whole context into
account, though, including anxiety about the
acquisition of information. Non-directive teaching
is no solution, since it just cuts down the
information to minimise loss [a classic
recommendation of the 'new pedagogy' in HE].
Ultimately, there is a relation
between the language used and the social system.There are some technical
problems, for example whether to maximize
effective transmission by minimising redundancy,
or minimise loss by incorporating lots of
redundancy.The
traditional relation to language is a serious
barrier to either technique.What happens is redundancy in a particular
academic sense—‘like musical variation on certain
themes’, rather than ‘conscious and calculated
repetition’, or ‘ellipsis by omission and
understatement in contrast to [technical]
concision’ (43).
Pedagogy is disdained in
universities, seen as too elementary.The taught are seen via
‘cultural ethnocentrism’ (43).The ideal student is ‘defined by a superior
knowledge’, producing scathing views of real
students as philistines.The
poor results of professional communication
invariably leads to blaming the students.A kind of mutual
adjustment follows rather than an attempt to
actually improve communication.
It is not just a matter of
unequal knowledge and expertise, but different
practices, based on different interests in gaining
qualifications.Teachers
commonly emphasise values, interest, relation to
culture ‘in brief,…the
forms’, while ‘students expect to be supplied with
the content’, including emotional aspects (44).Students expect
professors to be gurus, to teach wisdom, to
possess charisma, and that is bound to lead to
disappointment ‘because the discourse on life
“neutralises” (in the phenomenological sense) that
upon which it speaks’ (44) [ that is it is always
technical and abstract discourse rather than the
engaged kind they seek?].Professors
do employee charismatic accents sometimes.In France philosophy
teachers have a peculiar status, because they
confer ‘the privilege of the extra mundane world’
(45).Such a view
promotes commitment ‘to the values of cultural
apprenticeship’ (45), producing a mutual
ideology—‘a polemical relation to the values of
the partner and at the same time an ideological
relation on the part of the partners to its own
values’ (45).
Class ethnocentrism
This is the ‘hidden spring’ of
academic life (46).Academic
languages are artificial, they not only offer a
technical relation to ideas, but deal in a ‘second
order language of allusions and cultural
complicities’ (46).This
is seen as ‘second nature to intelligent and
gifted individuals’, leading to natural divisions.Thus ‘academic
judgments…In reality
consecrate cultural privilege’ (46).This happens at the level of syntax as well
as vocabulary, producing ‘the system of
transposable mental
postures, themselves dependent on values which
dominate all experience’ (46).This is seen in ‘the nature of the relation
to words, reverential or free, borrowed or
familiar, sparing or intemperate’ (46).[This is supported by
observations of verbal behaviour at oral
examinations, which value ‘ease…facility of expression with off- handedness
of delivery and smoothness of tone’, instantly
detectable compared to the ‘forced ease which is
peculiar to working class and middle class
students…volubility
of delivery…discordance
of
tone’
(n8
46).Sometimes this
is good enough ‘not to be suspected of
self-seeking vulgarity’, helping to preserve the
‘prestigious fiction of an exchange [as] an end in
itself’ (46).
Academic language is a dead
language, distant from most people, not the mother
tongue for many. Not
to acknowledge this is to combine an ideological
version of open access with the reproduction of
inequalities, disguised as a different sets of
‘gifts’, but really reflecting social
inequalities.Teachers
often assume a prior cultural experience in their
students, putting working class students at a
disadvantage from the beginning.There is a split between language used in
universities and families, with university
language seen as not real.For
non-native speakers, this must lead to
‘dualization or... resigned submission to
exclusion’ (47).This
is especially so at secondary education: there is
a chance for university students to try again as
cultural apprentices.They
are still in some danger from the ‘illusion of
misunderstanding’, especially of words like
‘”dialectic”, “model”, “structure”,
“transcendental”, “ideology”’ (N9 47).Universities who recruit
without attention to communication will be as
exclusive as secondary schools.
The usual approach to teaching
causes pedagogical misunderstanding and focuses on
factors which are unmovable or beyond
action—generational differences, cultural gaps
between teacher and student.Youth culture can exaggerate these
misunderstandings, leading to ‘paternal ridicule’
of professors.Professors
have idealised pictures of themselves at the
students' age.The
adolescent subculture itself is unequally distant
from high culture: ‘upper class students manifest,
even in domains most distant from academic
orthodoxy, dispositions to erudition or
eclecticism, very close to the habits required or
favoured by the school’ (48).
[seems to put a class base to omnivorousness? ].
This general distance is sharpened by a
class distance, so that professorial disdain for
students is often simultaneously disdain for
working class or middle class adolescents [N 11
suggests that the rarity of working class students
makes this worse in universities, compared to
primary schools, 49].
The dominant language can seem
familiar.It is often
surrounded by familiar words or found in contexts
that produce ‘an impression of familiarity’ (49).When employed in the
pedagogical situation, it draws elements from its
institutional setting, from organisational
separations between teachers and students, for
example.The rostrum,
professorial chair, the situation which focuses
attention on the lecturer, who is separated from
the audience, leaves only ‘dramatic monologue and
virtuoso exhortation’ (50), a delivery based on
‘intonation, diction…and
oratorical action’ (50).Dialogue
in such spaces is a fiction—‘questions to the
audience are often only oratorical' (50).There is little danger
of actual real participation as students note for
themselves.The
students therefore act as ‘the faithful at a
service, the answers are most often only
responses’ (50).
The hold of the system on
student is apparent when they are asked what
reforms they would like to see.They can often not conceive of any
innovation, but suggest simple technical
improvements, like microphones to help them hear
the teacher.This
conservatism lies underneath the occasional
‘appeal, in a suitably pious voice, for greater
freedom in pedagogical exchanges’ (51).Revolutionary students
are either utopians or traditionalists.Those rare advocates of
a circular layout can sometimes only see in them a
possibility for student cruelty: ‘the outward
passivity of students does not exclude a masked
aggression’ (52), a classic outcome of seeing the
professor as a parent.On
the whole, students seek personal safety [N16 says
there are lots of risky emotions in being a
student, for example those based on past rankings
in school].
Misunderstanding combines with
‘the fiction of the absence of misunderstanding’
in the logic of the system (53).The attitudes of teachers and students only
express this logic.Students
develop ‘verbal reverberation’ which leads to the
overestimate of the effectiveness of communication
and masks misunderstanding, which explains their
fondness for didactic methods rather than ‘genuine
dialogue’ (54).
The lecture and the
dissertation mirror each other, as does the
‘professorial solo and solitary prowess at the
exams’ (54).The
university offers ‘programmes without horizons or
shores’ leading to ‘essays [which are] tests of
cultural manners judged according to diffuse
criteria’ (54).All
this would be exposed by a demand for adequate
communication.It
would not be in the interests of teachers who are
not trained to do this, and students would see it
as more work.Professors
would look like mere teachers.Students keep up their defences by
‘emulating professorial rhetoric…False generalities…Prudent
approximations of the “not even wrong”’ (55).[N17 says tasks are
communicated ‘in a quasi- explicit fashion in
preparation classes {for the preparation year}’
leading to maxims such as ‘take the middle path,
avoid writing nothing under the pretext of knowing
nothing’].Those
students who are best at deciphering rewrite the
lecture, avoiding any ‘unmistakable nonsense’ and
produce ‘a finished batch of semantic atoms,
chains of mechanically linked words’ (55).Essays indicate a
discourse designed to prevent stark choices, one
which needs markers to make judgements [more hints of the omnivore?] The
results are seen in the well-known problems of
marking a batch of middling essays, with
professors trying to produce ‘a verdict of
indulgence tainted with scorn’ (56).Professors also claimed to be marking
general and authentic qualities of persons.
The system is designed to
produce ‘echolalia’ to cover misunderstanding.Many students cannot
define common terms, and have to produce
‘reciprocal alibis’ (56).Familiarity
will do rather than comprehension.Essays can offer a ‘constellation of
semantic impressions through mutual consonance and
dissonance…[Terms]…shoulder each other up’
(56).There is no
interest in analysis because the right impression
will do.[N18 refers
to the tests of definitions described in more
detail below, and noticed that student
difficulties are sometimes explained away by insisting that they really
understand but only in context.I am reminded of a common explanation of
abysmal performance in examinations as showing the
untoward effects of stress and artificiality].
Students acculturate rather
than learn.They
employ ‘allusion and ellipsis’ in their essays
(58).Teachers seldom
try to find out what students mean.Essays are seen as the ‘”pointing to” of
another possible discourse, the complete knowledge
and comprehension of which the teacher alone
possesses’ (58).Students
assume that teachers will fill in.
This leads to inevitable
‘contradictions and dissociations’ (58).Teachers teach ‘fictive
subjects’ and expect students to approximate to
them. If not, the
student is to blame.This
clearly discourages authenticity.Only ‘gifted students’ approximate to the
ideal.Teachers are
always able to blame students if there are
misunderstandings on the rare occasion they
appear, for example by deploring the decline in
standards—‘a rite of reassurance’ (16).Poor standards of
students are inevitable.Nothing
can be done.Their
poor performance justifies the system [much as
deviants make the rest of us feel better in
Durkheim].Students
are forced to try effective communication if they
are to succeed and even then they feel unworthy
and that they ought not to be there, rather than
insisting on their right to understand.They see themselves as
impostors compared to the ideal student—one
student is quoted on the fear of ridicule (61),
including ridicule by students pretending to be
ideal.[n 22 says
this fear is exaggerated if others people’s grades
are unknown].
Teachers and students require
complementary attitudes to develop.These are seen displayed in spatial
arrangements again—professorial distance protects
him and means that real communication is
impossible.The
professor addresses no one in particular,
operating with a ‘diffuse responsibility [which]
becomes irresponsibility of everyone’ (62).Teachers are insecure
because their role requires ‘successive acts of
virtuosity’ (62).Professorial
language is the best way to maintain distance—it
is institutional but it appears to be personal
expression.It is a
useful resource even when teachers physically mix
with their students.Professorial
tone is important too—professors talk upon rather
than about things, and pose as a neutral expert on
any topic.This is
associated with a charismatic style, offering
education through inspiration, often using
incantation to put the student ‘in a state fit to
receive grace’ (63).Ceremonial
oratory supports this view [as in our degree
ceremonies—the vicar speaks and then the
academics].The word
seduces students into confirming academic culture.This is quite different
to the rational use of speech [n 24 says the use
of the language in lectures only confirms the
status of the professor as an extraordinary
person.The tone is
like assuming that the audience is following a
sophisticated comedy {cf role distance}.Ceremonial language
often shows an allusion to an assumed shared
understanding].
Students are outmanoeuvred and
have to resort to a ‘rhetoric of despair’,
‘magical use of language’, ‘mechanical recitation
of ideas’ (64).There
is a prophylactic use of prudence and over
relativising [so things are not even wrong].There are frequent
hommages, and signs of ‘propitiatory ritual’, and a‘despairing
imitation of…academic
language’ (64).Academic
language displays ‘verbal exhibition' and it is no
mystery why even practical classes get taken over
and turned into lectures, despite a common
tendency to blame material conditions like an
unsuitable room.
Universities themselves
authorise ‘transmission by speech’ and valorise it
compared to say the correction of scripts or the
organisation of student work (65).For these minor tasks are often done by
assistants, so an organisational hierarchy mimics
an intellectual one.Professorial
scorn for rational techniques is picked up by
students too.
Students are also wordsmiths,
and an ‘aptitude to manipulate the academic
language remains the principal factor in success
at exams’ (66).This
attitude is linked to family culture.It helps if the student
has had an apprenticeship in ‘decipherment and the
management of complex structures’ (66) [N26 refers
to Bernstein and
elaborated code].Verbal
expression gets better as social class does, and
verbalisation of experience is the key, as Sartre
recognised.
We do need to rationalise
pedagogical language.We
need clear criteria, ‘diminishing the role of
manners and of diffuse savoir faire’ (67).Teachers must see that
academic language is ethnocentric and that
students are influenced by their social origins.They must make ‘explicit
all the presuppositions of the academic
manipulation of language’ (67) [impossible
surely].Professorial
language should be able to define its terms and
refer to actual evidence [N26 says that the
intentions are crucial here, whether we want to
explain or mystify].Professors
need actual information about actual students.There should be open to
potential interruption and challenge, to demands
to explain the code, and not operate with a taken
for granted academic language.
Bourdieu and Passeron admit
that this is ‘utopian under present circumstances’
(68).[N27 gives
examples of resistance to clear criteria, by
students, especially traditional middle class
Parisians.For this
reason, provincial students are more likely
targets for reform].The
old dichotomies and divisions, between student and
teachers, make it difficult to see the need to
reform the whole system.Secret
complicity needs to be exposed.Complicities are never explicit, more a
matter of mutual bad faith.Students
and teachers share the same objective end, both
trade security for better information, and both
engage in ritual exhortations to activism
[activist teaching that is].In practice, both participants prefer
comfort.
Appendix
It is possible to classify
methods of teaching according to the extent to
which they involve machines.Most teaching is person to person.When
students were asked to design the ideal classroom
and discuss it, they displayed an overwhelming
support for face to face.Machines
were only used to enhance, such as microphones.Printed media were
particularly unpopular.Some
student proposals included some tokenist SF
devices such as avant-garde architecture, but
still wanted traditional lectures.They sought new comforts.There was very little interest in changing
pedagogical relationships, more interest in things
like hearing and seeing better.The proposal for some circular forms
produce some of the quotes mentioned earlier,
where teachers are expected to be spectacles and
to receive student aggression—one student
particularly wanted the teacher to be a scorpion
in a circle of burning twigs, while another wanted
the teacher to feel powerless and alone.Others wanted lectures
to be entertainment or
spectacle.
Bourdieu,
P.,
Passeron, J – C., De Saint Martin, M.‘Students and the
Language of Teaching’
This was based on a substantial
inquiry undertaken in 1962-63, designed to test
the general notion of linguistic misunderstanding
that the team had formulated.It was not intended to be a representative
sample of students, because it was explanatory,
although it turned out to be fairly typical in
terms of social origin, age and sex of university
students in these subjects.Sociology
and philosophy students formed the core, although
there were some other students in other courses as
well.11 French
universities’ students were surveyed.
The team chose to research real situations rather
than experimental ones, and to recreate academic
conditions.There is
also a mention of a project to test teacher
judgement and the relevance of assessment criteria
[although this barely appears here].The real situation avoids using standard
attitude tests and thus of ‘tautologically
defining the measurement attributes as the object
of the test which measures them’ (80).Actual exercises were
used instead.The
results were bound to be asymmetric because in
real academic life, weak performance is over
emphasized.The basic
argument is that the language of teaching is
differentially remote from other domains of
language [that is ordinary language spoken in
different social classes].
They tested several domains of
vocabulary, and levels of linguistic behaviour
[see the details in the appendix below].Linguistic behaviour is
interesting, and ranged from comprehension through
active manipulations ‘like the explicit
consciousness of polysemia’, through to picking
correct definitions (81).The
research was to see if the skills were linked to
eventual success.[Lower
down, we are told that detecting polysemia is
closely linked to being able to do academic
analysis, for example].The
words chosen showed either a high frequency in
actual lectures, or were selected because they
were both frequent and undefined, suggesting that
the word should be understood.
The results do show the
importance of linguistic misunderstanding in
higher education and its relation to academic
success.They reveal
lots of ‘imperfect comprehension of academic
language, and even of common language’ (81).50% of errors happen to
be the mean point, producing what looked like a
normal distribution after all.There were very common confusions between
words such as ‘disinterestedness’ and
‘disinterest’, (81% error rate).Some words produced very odd associations
for some students, such as one who defined
‘epistemology’ as ‘the study of memoirs, journals
and correspondence’ (82) [some confusion with
epistles possibly?].Social
characteristics were linked to variations in test
results.[N 2 on page
82 shows that the definitional test was the most
useful in simulating academic work, because it
corresponded most closely to marking criteria.Correct definitions seem
to be important even where marking criteria were
made explicit {compare this with the emphasis on
‘presentation’ in our modest studies of marking
criteria}.Performance
on definitional tests were also the most clearly
linked to social origins.
[For example of the research,
see the full table on page 83] [and below]
The ability to manipulate
academic language is important to success
especially in literary studies.The cultural heritage passed on by the
family ‘never ceases to operate…[Because it]…Furnishes
a syntax [as well as a vocabulary]’. This heritage
is passed on ‘by osmosis’ (84) [they really like
this metaphor!] Having a useful syntax seems
effortless to the ‘cultivated class’ who like to
think of themselves as gifted.
[One important finding is the
role of selection].Students
have already been unequally selected before they
get to university, according to their social class
of origin.This
produces serious problems for conventional
statistical analysis if it tries to adopt
‘exclusive’ definitions [of matters such as social
class].Social class
has already defined the sample.So many working class students have already
been eliminated.[N
1, page 85, says examinations have already
stressed the need to write well, for example,
especially those used to select for the Ecole
Normale—anecdotal evidence seems to be used here
though].
Selection in fact has a
particular effect, and heavily selected working
class students can compete with culturally
advantage to upper class students, and can
outperformed the less selected middle class
students.It is the
middle class group who has benefited most from
university expansion (87) [which has implications
for the democratisation of universities as we
shall see].Successful
selected working class students have come from
only slightly less unfavourable families in the
first place.There
seems to be something about selection that
produces cases where ‘the correlation between
results and social background will be completely
reversed’ (88), especially if working class
students live in Paris: there they enjoy a
suitable cultural context, but they also face more
rigorous selection.As
the less selected middle class students are the
ones who do worst, less stringent selection would
only restore the relation between class and
academic results, however [in other words,
students will find it easier to get to university
despite their class of origin, but they will still
face the effects of having unequal stocks of
cultural capital—reducing the effects of class in
recruitment does not reduce the effects of class
in attainment].[N1,
page 90 suggests it is this opening of access to
middle class students that has produced a commonly
noted ‘decline in standards’].
Sex produces inequalities as
well.In the tests
they did, males got better results than females.Before we lead to any
conclusions though, the females in the sample were
also more likely to find themselves channelled
into literary subjects, they had been less
selected, and they were ‘less rationally oriented’
(91).[Before any
feminists get too excited, I think what the team
are arguing here is that they have been less
determined to succeed, which arises from their
having to face less selection].Indeed, the whole point of that discussion,
is to show that conventional analysis usually
explains these gaps in terms of some natural
inequalities, but for them, their fully
explanatory model ‘can account for, if it is
applied completely, all the empirical data which
the most systematic multivariate analysis would
leave unexplained, save by recourse to an account
according to the “natural inequalities between the
sexes”’ (91).The
female students in the sample were different from
the male ones according to their social origins,
the type of studies they chose, and their academic
background, especially whether they had received
training in the classical languages, Latin and
Greek—and all these are connected to academic
success.
So, if multivariate analysis
systematically claims to have discovered diverse
variables, it should be able to show ‘other
effective relations’ beneath the connections
between sex and results.However,
a gap remains between the genders even after such
analysis, which tempts people to think of natural
inequalities.There
is undoubtedly a gap, and it does seem to remain
constant in various educational organizations.However, the analyses of
esteem shows some interesting anomalies—for
example, lycee [high school] girls do as well as
boys, and the team thinks this is because they
face equal patterns of selection.However, when it comes to university, girls
are less strongly selected at entrance,
principally because they choose courses in the
arts faculty.They
make these choices as a result of the ‘social
definition of “feminine” qualities’ (92), shared
by the girls themselves and their families—these
clearly affect their ‘choice’ (93).What this shows is that the explanatory
model showing a link between the degree of
selection and degree of success, as well as other
social factors, still holds up.As further evidence, girls who are taking
classical languages do better in them than boys,
because fewer are selected to do classics in the
first place—the ‘rare few
who go against the current seem to have to satisfy
more demands’ (94).[Ain’t
that the truth!] [N1, 94, says the girls tend to
take more supplementary courses].Combining selection to university with
selection to arts courses specifically is
sufficient to explain the differences between boys
and girls on the tests.[Table
eight, page 95, does not actually give figures,
but symbols indicating the strength of the
relationship].
Disciplines chosen also have an
effect.Students on
mixed courses show lower results than those taking
single disciplines.Those
taking preparatory courses have lower results than
those taking first year trial courses at
university [the strange French system of a
foundation year, almost, to try out university
life.Triallists are
more heavily selected, and face further heavy
selection at the end of their first year].This is nothing to do
with natural qualities or gifts.Triallists are more likely to come from
upper class and middle class backgrounds, there
are more likely to have studied classical
languages, they are older, and they are more
likely to have been high school pupils.
In the extensive discussion
that follows on this point, certain points appear.The test of polysemia is
related to an ‘academic, analytic attitude’.Tests used to select
Triallists overemphasise issues of definition.Beneath the elaborated
categories in the official criteria, which offer
‘fine – grain differentiation’, there seemed to be
three basic categories—‘”brilliant”, “mediocre”,
and “worthless”’ (98).Philosophy
students do better than the sociology students who
do better than those on mixed courses.The most liberal courses
in terms of recruitment most faithfully reproduced
the institutional hierarchy.Philosophy students are more mixed than the
others—they include students who have chosen them
despite modest achievements, because they are more
prestigious—but also produce the most polarised
results.Sociology
students tend to be older, more privately
educated, and to lack of preparation for a
scientific discipline.Students
are mixed courses tend to be less committed to
university life and success.Sociology can ‘shelter’ students who are
not well adapted to academic life, often upper
class boys.Gender
differences are stronger in philosophy, where male
students seemed definitely more committed to
success: the lack of commitment needed to
undertake mixed studies reduces the gender
advantage of males.
So, a complex structure is
revealed, but this can be explained [unlike the
Bennett study], because ‘a complete system of
relations commands the meaning of each particular
relation’ (101) [hints again of our old friend a
structure in dominance].Multivariate
analyses is inadequate to reveal this structure,
and produces either ‘aporia, or…The reification of pure abstract relations’
(101).Social groups
[by definition?] are defined by a ‘totality of
relations which they maintain with their past and…Present situation’
(101).We therefore
need to grasp the ‘totality of components of an
academic career’ (101) [this notion of totality is
starting to look a bit Marxist as well, with
notions of surfaces and depths].Thus, for example, past academic
achievements, especially the knowledge of
classical languages are ‘more strongly linked than
any other criteria to the high rate of success’
(102), whatever the test.[But
these past achievements themselves are linked to
the usual social demographics plus the effects of
a selection process—so we can’t just equalise
achievements by teaching lots of kids Latin].
Ancient languages are
important, but not as a form of mental training,
more as a medium for other social relations.Classic students are
those who conform most closely to academic
demands.They are
also already the most highly selected working
class children.There
are no intrinsic virtues—for example, Latin and
Greek together are associated more with success
than just studying Latin alone.Even language based tests, such as
identifying polysemes or malapropisms, shown no
advantage to Latin scholars.Latin and Greek seemed to be associated
most with ‘verbal ease’, but this is also an
effects of having been heavily selected.Further, classicists are
seen as elite pupils, the best ones [so doing
classics gives you elite status, a point well
recognized by Coleridge and Kay Shuttleworth who
insisted on training teachers taking Latin.Incidentally, Kay
Shuttleworth also predicted that a knowledge of
Latin would help trainee schoolmasters understand
educated middle class speech, and not see it as
arbitrary, and church liturgy].It is the pedagogical context of classic
studies rather than any intrinsic skills [N 2 on
page 103 says that pupils who have studied the
classics at non selective private schools seem to
receive no advantage.N
3 says that studying the classics polarises
student results rather than providing a smooth and
constant advantage, and this can be seen even with
medical students—Greek helps the best, but
facility with Greek is also associated with the
worst!As a result,
requiring Latin and Greek could be abolished as a
form or requirement of university entrance, but
given that the admissions tutors like ‘verbal
ease’, which they see as a sign of conformity, it
is likely that classics will still provide an
advantage].
The key effect of social
background is that it provides a familiarity with
the language of ideas.It
is not just a matter of material conditions like
income.Income does
up in the statistics as an effect, but only
because income is also related to the
qualifications of the head of household, so the
real effects are ‘nearly exclusively cultural’
(105).Mothers and
grandparents also have effects.Occasionally, something in a family can
overturn the other effects of class
membership—thus, for example upper class sons do
not receive a smooth constant advantage, but
display bimodal results.Here
there seems to be an effective cultural
orientations and other suspected variables [N 3
says that the sons of upper class families
sometimes decide to squander their cultural
heritage, 105] if those sons exploit the cultural
advantage this can help them gain entrance to a
high school rather than a private school, and then
definite advantages follow.Generally,
the tests of knowledge of the ‘language of
humanism’ [a measure of familiarity with
established legitimate culture?] do show greater
returns to cultural heritage in general, with
fewer effects of decisions to exploit or squander
(106). Those working class children who do Latin
also show the effects of unusual family settings,
and their decision to exploit their heritage and
to persevere.This
can push them past middle class kids in terms of
success.It is worth
remembering, however, that working class children
take Latin three times less often than students
‘from leisured classes’ (107).
So there is no single
determinant of success.There
are close links between success, a successful
academic past, and social background.Even so, conditions can
be overcome by working class children choosing
particularly favourable options early on,
particularly Latin and Greek.However, there is still a very uneven
uptake.Is a mistake
to look for causal connections, since these
background factors all need to be mediated.We cannot reconstruct
career paths from multivariate analysis of
factors.We need to
grasp real experiences and how they are ‘concrete,
unitary and endowed with meaning’ (108).Nonetheless, class
situation is ‘the point from which every possible
view proceeds and upon which no other view is
possible’ (108).
The appendices give
considerable details of both the sample and the
questionnaires.As
examples of the tests used, students were required
to:
(A)Underline
words which are used improperly in sentences
provided.Instructions
tell students not to discuss but to focus on
examples of misuse.[The
sentences look extremely high powered—such as ‘The
sequence of axioms flowing from one another deep
actively, mathematical reasoning is no less
apodictic than the Aristotelian syllogism’, or
‘The civil law is the palladium of property’].
(B)Define
words, such as antimony, epistemology, Manichaeism
(C)Enumerate
all the possible meanings of words such as
attribute, function, realism
(D)Choose
synonyms for words such as stumble, disposition,
emetic
(E)Choose
definitions from a list of words such as
broaching, milling, fallow, counter point,
litotes, scumble.
The team admit that these are
very difficult words, but insist they are used in
teaching, often without definition.
Lectures were analysed and the
frequency of words was noted, but also the
idiomatic use of key terms.For
example, in sociology lectures, words like
functions stratification and conjuncture, in
philosophy antinomy, epistemology.From language common to both, terms such as
‘contrary, virtual, generic, participation, image
and realism’ (123) there were also terms such as
‘Manichaeism, extension, attribute, apodictic,
transcendental, valorise, acceptance, axiom,
dubitative [sic] productivity, disinterestedness’.More general difficult
terms included ‘cadastral [the translators offer
no equivalent], numinous, neuropath, climacteric’.The team also used a
standard test of comprehension, a test of
technical terms from the arts, and a list of
proper names and classical humanities which
students were invited to recognise [such as
Helen]: these are all used in manuals or selection
tests for secondary schools.
Results:
Bourdieu et al
Melbourne Seminar Papers
Tab1e 4. Results on
the five exercises by the main variables
(medians of distributions)
Exercise Exercise Exercise Exercise Exercise
I II III IV V
MalapropismDefinition Polysemia Concrete Lang Language of
Humanism
Negative Positive Positive Negative Negative
Score ScoreScore Score Score
0-14 5-24 0-13 0-15 0-9
Median Median Median Median Median
Type of Study .
Phi1osophy 6.9 13.6 7.1 7.1 5.0
Sociology 7.1 11.7 7.7 6.8 4.7
Composite 8.6 10.9 6.4 7.0 4.9
Pre1iminary Yr 9.3 11.1 5.6 8.3 5.4
Prep. students 8.0 11.9 7.0 7.0 4.2
Sex
Ma1es 7.2 12.6 7.2 6.5 4.8
Females 8.3 11.2 6.5 7.6 4.9
Social Origin
Farm Labourer 8.1 11.7 6.5 6.8 5.6
Manual Worker,
wage-earner 8.0
11.5 6.3 7.0 4.6
Artisan,shpkper 8.1. 11.0 7.0 7.2 5.0
Middle c1asses 8.0 11.5 6.7 7.0 4.9
Upper classes 7.4 12.5 7.2 7.0 4.7
Secondary School
Lycee7.812.37.85.85.8
College 8.8 11.56.3 7.1 5.2
Private Instit.7.6 11.0 6.6 6.9 5.0
Type of Secondary
Education
No Greek/Latin 8.3 11.4 6.6 6.9 5.2
Latin8.1 11.2 6.6 7.0 5.1
Latin & Greek 7.2 12.2 7.3 6.7 4.3
Senior(?)Academic
Success
Weak 7 8.11.2 6.5 7.3 5.1
Medium 7.0 13.1 7.8 6.7 4.7
Strong 6.1 14.0 8.2 5.7 4.2
Use of(?)dictionary
Very weak 7.5 11.5 7.3 6.7 5.2
Weak 7.5 12.1 6.8 6.5 4.7
Medium 8.2 11.2 6.6 7.3 5.2
Strong 7.7 14.2 6.8 7.1 4.8
Very strong 7.2
11.2 6.7 6.7 4.5
Universities
Paris6.6 13.7 7.8 6.9 4.4
Lyon6.513.06.56.65.3
Bordeaux9. 1 10.5 7.5 6.3 4.5
Nancy8.310.56.97.55.2
Dijon7.1 12.2 6.6 6.8 4.7
Clermont8.111.76.85.85.2
Toulouse9.39.56.48.65.3
Montpellier8.8 11.4 6.0 7.2 5.0
Caen7.812.56.66.04.3
Lille9.010.75.66.45.3
Rennes8.310.07.87.64.9
AGGREGATE8.011.66.86.95.0
If I understand this
correctly, the median score on each test is
given – showing that half the students fall
below that score. If the median shows a score of
about half marks, it shows that half the sample
got below 50% etc. (or only half got in the top
half). This is more or less what you would
expect with a test designed to discriminate, of
course –but these weren’t? All students could
have got 100%?