Notes on: Ellis
C .& Rawicki, J (2014) More than
Mazel. Luck and Agency in
Surviving the Holocaust. Journal
of Loss and Trauma 19
(99--110). DOI:
10.1080/15325024.2012.738574
Dave Harris
This is a very interesting piece to compare with
the 2006 article,
because again it involved stories of survivors of
tragedies, in this case the Holocaust.
Interestingly, personal accounts are in conflict
with academic accounts of the factors that led to
survival, but Ellis and Rawicki are much less
inclined to dismiss the academic accounts, even
though they seem to offer classic analytic
realism. [NB Rawicki is the 'Jerry' in the text,
Ellis writes the piec in the first person
singular] The problem is that the survivors
interviewed tend to attribute their survival to
factors based on 'luck', while Ellis is much more
interested in what might be seen as background
variables, of the kind associated with classic
sociological analysis of samples of survivors. The
abstract says that the conclusion merely attempts
'considering both positions in compassionate
collaborative research' (99) but there are clearly
familiar tensions.
In more detail, a survivor, Jerry is quoted as
saying that he '"survived because of luck, pure
and simple"', while Ellis wonders 'what lies
beneath that statement'. In the early work,
statements by survivors were taken as
straightforward expressions with nothing lying
beneath them. The particular survivor is part of a
sample of 45 others, and the researchers used 'the
process of collaborative witnessing in which we
freely exchange ideas and work back and forth over
an extended period to write and explore concrete
stories of [their] experiences' (100). However
they also wanted to explore 'the broader
literature… Connecting it to Jerry's stories'. In
particular, there are 'possible consequences
regarding the lack as the sole explanation of
survival'. Jerry's own account represents a
different understanding, and must be considered in
'compassionate collaborative research'. Jerry is
by no means a simple survivor, however because he
has written a novel, several stories, and even
'scholarly essays and stories' (101).
Ellis says that in response to Jerry's assertion
on the centrality of luck, 'there may be more'.
Jerry picks out some examples of chance events in
other people's stories, but Ellis is still
convinced that 'other factors may have worked
together with luck. That is what I'm interested
in' (102). Luck avoids further explanation of
experiences and 'limits our understanding of how
survival took place'. It is not at all clear who
she means when she says our understanding — maybe
academics who have analysed survival. The point is
that it is not just the personal understandings
and emotions of the survivors that seem to be at
stake.
Jerry tries to persist and 'seems adamant
about his position, even while he acknowledges my
logic' (103) which makes Ellis wonder why it is so
important to see luck 'as the all-encompassing
explanation for survival'. This is an example of
classic academic violence, of course. She
specifies other factors including how fast people
can run or how quickly they size up situations.
Jerry persists that it was only luck. Ellis
advances a quotation attributed to Thomas
Jefferson that hard work seems to increase luck,
and 'a Roman philosopher' who agrees that
preparation is needed to meet opportunities. Again
Jerry denies that he was better prepared. He
worries that the stories might make him look like
a hero and distract attention from those who died.
He doesn't not want to claim any exceptional
skill, or divine intervention for that matter.
Ellis cannot forget 'Kushner's admonition... that
to do justice to the richness and complexity of
the Holocaust we must pay attention to ordinary
people's constructions of their lives', and asks
herself why she wants to 'expand Jerry's point of
view'. The answer is that she is 'committed as a
scholar to explore the complexity of survivors
memories' and wants 'to advance a more complex
understanding of survival'. She realises that this
risks 'invalidating the perceptions and memories
of survivors' (104) and offers some
multidimensional account where 'luck and agency
can go hand-in-hand'.
She explores more detail of Jerry's memories.
Again he resists claiming any agency, although the
stories he told showed that physical health
ability was important, as were linguistic talents
and quick responses, including being able 'to read
others correctly… [And] to act in a way that
others read him in ways he intended' There are
some brilliant examples of Jerry's
resourcefulness, including pretending to share the
anti-Semitism of Germans. His cautious skill
depends on what Ellis calls '"ethnographic
sensibilities"', while he calls them '"being
circumspect and pragmatic"'. He acknowledges that
others had helped save him. However, he denies
that his actions were planned and concludes that
luck was the main factor, even affecting any
agentive qualities he might have had. There is a
particularly interesting story summarised
demonstrating this pages 106 –7, where an accident
of recognition saved the day. Ellis says this is
'convincing, but also quotes another story
involving considerable skill in being able to
blend in with crowds, and maintaining his nerve
and courage.
She went back to some of the literature about luck
and survival and realised that 'Jerry's
explanation fits with the canonical response of
survivors' (108). That includes the accounts by
Levi, and a host of others cited on p 108. She
agrees that 'good fortune' was clearly involved,
but suggest that luck is really 'a moral
explanation' negating the idea of any superiority
among survivors, a form of '"narrative humbling" —
"a humbling before the dead"', avoiding any form
of blame directed at victims.
This leaves Ellis with a dilemma. She does not
want to downplay these accounts, or evaluate them,
or imply that there was any superiority attached
to survivors, but 'at the same time, I do want to
consider some of the factors that may have
intersected with luck' (109), insisting that this
would not in any way divert from the enormity of
the crimes. We should not be 'fearful to unpack
"luck' and would miss aspects of the story
including 'kindness, altruism, and organisation.
We have to offer explanations like this to defeat
the view that 'the Holocaust is mystical,
unexplainable, unspeakable and beyond human
reason' (109).
Some survivors even argued that '"the worst
survived, the selfish, the violent… the
collaborators"' [quoting Levi], and Ellis finally
tackles the difficult issue of 'survivor guilt'.
Levi has certainly said that some of those who
survived stole from their own companions, for
example. An insistence on luck would be 'a way not
to condemn or judge those who survived as well as
a way not to judge or condemn those who died'.
Other researchers have specified other factors,
and they are summarised at length on page 110.
Here, classic academic analysis seems to be valued
[one at least involved a questionnaire]
Factors include various cultural conditions and
resources as well as personal characteristics.
There were national and geographical differences,
as well as 'municipal – level factors' including
the loyalties of local policeman and population.
All this provided different experiences depending
on context — '90% of the Jewish population were
killed in Poland and Germany, while 20% were
killed in Italy'. The existence of '"informal
communities" (111) also seemed important, not
least in providing information and a 'survival
ideology of living in the moment'. Here, Ellis
draws on memoirs of survivors.
Still others have talked about personal and social
factors which can be 'separated into internal and
external and into agentic and non-agentic
categories' — age, gender, factors like
determination and intelligence health skill and
knowledge. Survivors sometimes give more credit to
these external factors which include luck, while
some did not offer an explanation for survival at
all. Age was important so that 'the death rate for
Jewish children was close to 90%, while it was
about 67% for adults', demonstrating 'a
curvilinear effect on survival'. Looking and
sounding Arian also 'significantly increased
survival chances' [citing another study]. Gender
had effects — male Jews who had been
circumcised were more easily detected, and more
often to be chosen for slave labour or death
camps, while women were more able to participate
in informal communities. However, women also
suffered sexual violence and were sometimes
singled out for execution 'as a way to prevent
future generations from being born' (112).
Financial resources helped survival, for example
by purchasing false documents or bribing guards.
Occupational skills were helpful in survival if
they were needed by the Germans, and they also
were sometimes woven more into social networks.
Physical health, especially when contrasted to the
variety of work required was also important.
Psychological profiles have also been identified,
and factors here include 'realism — which means
realistically assessing the situation and
environment — presence of mind and the ability to
make decisions instantly, and an absolute
determination to live'. Other factors include
'perseverance, initiative and ingenuity…
Optimism', while Lyndon specifically argues that
'survival was primarily a social process, with
people depending on each other for life itself
(112). Sometimes 'mental mechanisms of defence
were important… Estrangement from self, psychic
splits, and blocking mechanisms' survivors use
various languages and mention resources or
resilience, but the main focus appears to be on
'adaptive behaviours'.
Some survivors concede that these other factors
might have been important — Levi for example, who
seemed to refer to what looks like cultural
capital, as opposed to the everyday familiarity
with work that workers and peasants had..
Nevertheless Levy says that culture saved him,
especially as ability to make links with the past
and construct imagined academic accounts. There
are also those who stress spiritual escapes. All
of these mention 'the importance of "meaning
making" (113). [Some used a questionnaire].
Of course we can only work with how survivors
remember and tell their stories 'and [how] social
scientist interpret it'. Both are going to be
partial. On meeting Jerry to summarise what she
had found, she asks whether or not his life was
made meaningful. He says that he didn't really
think much about meaning in those days. Ellis
explains that '"meaning" is how social scientists
and historians think and talk, not how survivors
do — unless they are academics'. So she asked a
different question — whether or not he thought of
death. Jerry's answer reminded her of a passage in
Levi. Jerry adds 'holding back tears' that they
were too many other things to think about.
Suddenly Ellis can 'experience a new level of
understanding' some empathy with the young boy.
She can feel the emotion. She also has visual
images 'from viewing Holocaust movies,
documentaries, and photos'. She realises she might
have been insensitive, that Gerry would have found
it hard to maintain a sense of agency [certainly
not in the fully entitled American sense]. He
clearly realise that the skills he possessed did
not save others, and that whatever they did, they
faced random death. Surviving left him little time
to think, so it's not surprising that he saw
chance as the best explanation. She realises how
difficult it is with him, and understands his
preferences to opt for luck — so she 'more fully
understands Jerry's position now than when I began
this research' (115).
However she is still unwilling to subscribe to
luck as the main factor, partly because this would
reinforce the stereotype of Jews as passive and
incapable, denying the forms of resistance that
occurred. Jerry wants to honour all those who
resisted in whatever way they could. However he
still does not want to claim agency. Both try to
understand the other's stance. He realises that
social scientists '"have to take a holistic and
analytic view"', but he insists on his own
experience. Jerry 'hears the cries of his dead
relatives', while Ellis can 'hear the voices of
research scholars, and I want to offer them
complex explanations using the vocabulary we
share, such as meaning making and agency. Our
positions resist simplification' but indicate
instead complexity. There seems to be no anxieties
about social science being too 'aloof' or
oppressive.
She wants to honour and celebrate Jerry, but his
'explanation of luck seemed to camouflage so many
important details' [She has decided whjat is
important]. She must accept that Jerry does not
want to be a hero, but he is also not any more
cowardly and unscrupulous than others.
Note 3 addresses an obvious problem, that the
participants might be using the word 'luck'
differently. There are different meanings with
different stresses — some emphasise the power of
an outside force rather than chance. There is a
problem of 'conflation of categories in various
studies'. There are differences 'in philosophical
usage' so that fate refers to inevitable events,
good fortune something good happens to you, and
luck 'something that happens to you against the
odds'. (116). Chance is more impersonal, and
carries no obligation to be grateful. Other
meanings can be differentiated. For Jerry,
mystical happenings are mentioned, but primarily
he calls on 'luck and chance', with no sense of an
outside power choosing him: other survivors find
themselves having to answer why God might have
saved one and not another. Ellis is not just
working with how the participant defines things
then.
back to sociology of
leisure page
|
|