Notes on: Bagemihl, B (1999)
Biological Exuberance: Animal Homosexuality and
Natural Diversity New York: St Martin's Press
Dave Harris
[He has acknowledged lots of scientists and
obviously gathered a lot of information --far too
many examples piledon each other leaving little
chance to argue. Impossible to check or follow-up
all these activities, and a great deal of work is
done to generalise from them — birds may preen but
not mate, chimps may kiss and not groom, dolphins
can stimulate companions with sonic waves but may
not hug — and yet it all adds up to homosexual or
same-sex behaviour.
Why have plants been left out? Proust found far
more support for queer sexuality in the vegetal
kingdom, where you find very frequently
hermaphrodite or bisexual examples.Maybe it is
that they would be particularly difficult to
anthropomorphise — does japonica 'mate'. Animals
are close enough to us to provide that basis of
comparison.
A lot of this is cheerfully positivistic, although
there are some useful reservations about methods
introduced. I'm amazed that Barad could accept
this as authoritative, and even quote some of the
dubious numbers such as the 450 queer animal
species, without pointing out the obvious
objections. Presumably, it must be tactical again?
There clearly must be a context to the work in the
eternal struggle against those who think that
homosexuality is unnatural? There is some possible
special pleading saying that even the apparent
inferiority of lesbian couples in gaining the best
conditions can be compensated by superior
parenting, part of a general dithering about
whether females are better carers or not. The
overall point is to make us revise our stereotypes
and all that, but does any observer of the
contemporary human scene need that?
The most obvious problems the prevalence of so
many anthropomorphic terms, however, despite
reservations. Is the argument that animals exhibit
the same kind of sexual behaviours as humans,
equally cultural, equally based on imagination,
fantasy, elaborate social conventions of
attraction and repulsion, legitimacy and
illegitimacy, or is it that these animal
elaborations and diversities can still be
explained ultimately in biological and
environmental terms? Problems arise for me
particularly with terms like 'erotic', bearing in
mind Plummer's insistence on fantasy -- humans can
sexualise and desexualise anything he insists. Do
animals have anything like the fetishisms or
perversions arising from deliberate
experimentation? Or guilt,doubts, regrets, sexual
jealousy, feelings of inadequacy? In
particular, to take up one of Barad's points, do
Bighorn Sheep feel any sort of ethical
responsibility towards other male sheep that they
might be mounting?]
Cf with Barad (2012) 'Queer politics, if it is to
remain queer needs to be able to perform the
function of emptying [human] queerness of its
referentiality or positivity, guarding against its
tendency to concrete embodiment,and hereby
preserving queerness as a resistant relation
rather than as an oppositional substance' .The whole
emphasis in Bagemihl is on the functional
integration of animal queerness as it provides the
innovation needed for mechanical solidarity -- a
resistant relation requires a politics.
Introduction
it is a vast field and research is only in its
infancy. There are formidable challenges. He is
only considering cases where there is scientific
documentation which include scientific reports
observations by scientists ideally corroborated.
The result is he claims 'a uniform and verifiable
platform of data'. (1)The book focuses on animals
and birds.. Scientific researcher has gone on for
at least two centuries, but this is still a
beginning.
'Any account of homosexuality and transgender in
animals is also necessarily an account of human
interpretations of these phenomena. Because
animals cannot speak directly for themselves the
way people can, we must rely on human
observations… This presents both special
challenges and unique advantages… Certain
behaviour such as sexual acts can be observed
directly (and even quantified), which is often
extremely difficult, impossible, or unethical to
do in studies of sexuality among people… On the
other hand, we are in the dark about the
internal experiences of the animal participants:
as a result, the biases and limitations of the
human observer… Come to the forefront… The reverse
of what occurs in some studies of homosexuality
among people' (2). In particular we can get no
data about emotional and motivational states.
Contentious theories are chronic.
This is a 'unique historical moment'. We
need to present technical material to a
nonspecialist readership, but there is also a lack
of awareness among scientists. Hence this
structure of this book — full documentation in
notes and references, but the technical material
can be skipped.
Terminology is a particular problem. We have to go
between 'more accessible but overtly
anthropomorphic or loaded vernacular', and more
neutral but highly technical jargon on the other.
He has chosen homosexuality and same-sex rather
than gay and lesbian which are 'burdened with
human connotations (cultural, psychological,
historical, and/or political)' (3). Occasionally
these terms are used only for expedience, but they
are used increasingly by scientists in scholarly
publications, lesbian more than gay. There is some
discussion of trends, including transsexuality, in
the zoological literature 'with meanings largely
divorced from their human connotations'.
Even using the term homosexual can be
anthropomorphic, ignoring culture and
historical constructions with particular human
connotations. Other terms widely used including
monogamy or heterosexual, 'carry the same baggage
of human referents'. There may be more variation
within the species than between them. The
continued use of these terms is justified in
chapter 3. More generally, meanings are extended
explicitly and sometimes human connotations are
disavowed. The same problems applied to
heterosexual behaviour. Terminological debates
themselves are historical and turn on what counts
as appropriate, no terminology 'is entirely free
of human (cultural, historical, etc) associations'
(4), but the approach chosen here is not to invent
you terminology but use the usual terms having
offered 'careful qualification of their meanings
and understanding of their historical context,
such that they become uncoupled from the
anthropomorphic connotations'.
[Then the chapters are summarised. Particular
explanations are challenged in particular such as
that 'homosexuality might contribute to
heterosexual reproduction'(5). The book ends with
a call for a radical rethink, based on
'traditional beliefs about animal
homosexuality/transgender in indigenous cultures…
Western science has a lot to learn from aboriginal
cultures'. New sciences including chaos theory
might also be able to make a contribution. The
point is to make animal homosexuality explicable.
So examining sexual variants will 'offer a key to
a new way of looking at the world, symbolic of the
larger paradigms shifts'. It is 'rooted in the
basic facts', however, but there is also an
attempt to capture '"poetry" as well', where
diversity is 'one particular form of natural
beauty' and as such will inspire 'wonder, and…
awe'
Chapter 1
We start with Haldane's quote on the queer
universe. Apparently actual results show 130
different kinds of bird species worldwide
'literally are queer'[so being defined in terms of
sexual diversity']. Queer animals seek each other
out, caught each other, show 'tenderness and
affection towards one another', form pair bonds or
just 'meet briefly for sex'(12) [lots of pages
devoted to maps and pictures]. Same sex
relationships are common. Some transgendered
activity involves 'involves crossing or combining
characteristics of both males and females in their
appearance or behaviour'. Homosexual behaviour
'occurs in more than 450 different kinds of
animals' worldwide [but see the reservations
below] . It is not a singular phenomenon but
displays 'every conceivable variation'.
Homosexuality may mean just sex, but animals also
show 'major behavioural categories, courtship,
affection, sex, pair bonding, and parenting'.
Homosexuality is indeed 'defined as the same
activities when they occur between animals of the
same sex' however, 'the notion of identity is
inappropriate to ascribe to animals' so we can
only focus on behaviours, although we might
be able to 'describe individuals whose primary
"orientation" is towards animals of the same sex'.
Homosexuality can have 'specific meanings
independent of their human connotations' as well.
There can be bisexuality, 'when a particular
individual engages in both homosexual and
heterosexual activity'
Courtship behaviour involves advertising one's
presence to prospective mates. Mutual interest
might lead to mating and possibly pair bonding, or
specific sexual activity. Homosexual forms occur
'in nearly 40% of the mammals and birds in which
same-sex activity has been observed' (13). There
can be many forms, such as dances acrobatics,
poses [all of these are detailed], synchronised
displays. Sometimes they take place in specific
'display courts' for hetero and homo interactions
[lots more detail], sometimes to take advantage of
the characteristics of the light. There might be
different sounds, sometimes produced in unusual
ways. Again these are found in 'same-sex
courtship' (15). There may even be 'sonic
"foreplay"' where dolphins stimulate each other's
genitals with soundwaves'. Sometimes same-sex
courtship will only show a subset of these overall
behaviours. Some have special courtship patterns —
pirouette dances for homo ostriches, games of hide
and seek for female macaques doing 'lesbian
interactions'.
There is also a lot of touching which may not be
sexual involving the genitals but which still have
'clear sexual or erotic overtones' (16) —
'affectionate activities', and these are
found in a quarter of homosexual activity. 'Their
erotic nature in a same-sex context is usually
obvious: the two animals may be visibly sexually
aroused' [and in other cases?] and might lead to
homosexual copulation. Activities include grooming
or rubbing, and 'some animals also "kiss" each
other' (16), touching mouths or muzzles or bills
this 'can bear a startling resemblance to
the corresponding human activity', for example
there may be mouth-to-mouth contact '"passionate"
openmouthed kisses'. There may be hugging or
clasping. There may be play fights 'that have
erotic overtones' (17), and which do not display
particular physical violence. Elephants can become
sexually aroused, who could animals can mount each
other, sometimes it leads to sexual encounters.
Some animals bite or chew parts of their partners,
suck each other's nipples, pat each other's
bottoms, grasp each other's penises. There may be
unique behaviour such as necking in male giraffes.
Sometimes multiple animals are involved 'in near
"orgies" of bodily contact' (18). These display
'unabashedly sensual and playful aspects'
and this is 'actually reflected in the
descriptive names given to them by zoologists'
[reflected, note], sometimes with specially coined
nonsense words.
Overtly sexual behaviour is 'defined here as any
contact between two or more animals involving
genital stimulation'. Mounting is the most common
type, and it is found in homosexual contexts too.
Particular forms may be more common in homosexual
encounters. There may be unusually creative
mounting positions, especially with female
animals. Some animals 'mount their female partner
from the side rather than from behind; lateral
mounts also sometimes occur during heterosexual
interactions'. Group activity 'occurs in over 25
different species'.
There may be different kinds of genital contact,
including the insertion of fingers into the vagina
in lesbian orang utans, erect clitorises, fins and
tail flukes, wrapping and thrusting. There may not
be anal penetration — some dolphins have an
additional genital slit, penises may be inserted
into blowholes, or into specially created
concavities. There may be oral sex, including
'actual sucking of genitals' among male Bonabo, or
licking among sheep and vampire bats [loads of
examples all of them uncheckable, all asserted to
be the same kind of general behaviour]. There may
also be masturbation, stimulating genitals with
'the finger, hand, foot, flipper, or some other
appendage' (20), mutual masturbation, or dry
humping.
There may be significant pair bonds with animals
of the same sex — partners and companions,
depending on whether sex is involved or not.
Partnership involves 'a significant amount
of time with each other and they do similar
activities together' (21). Partnerships may become
consortships or sexual friendships, even
coalitions. Many will be 'exclusive or monogamous'
and ven lead to action to defend the pair bond.
There may be competition for homosexual/same-sex
partners, although some birds apparently also
promiscuously copulate and develop non-monogamous
pairs. Companionship is found in a number of
animals, sometimes involving 'younger same-sex
attendants' (22). Sometimes animals form a trio.
They may be bisexual with significant bonding or
sexual behaviour with same-sex partners, as well
as heterosexual trios, or same-sex trios. Pair
bonds seem to last as long as they do with
heterosexuals [at least in the example of the
Greylag Goose or Ocellated Antbirds]. Sometimes
the only pair bonds that occur will be homosexual.
'Same-sex pairs in many species (especially birds)
raise young together' (23) at least as competently
as heterosexual pairs. They managed to achieve
parenting in various ways e.g. both female
partners will copulate with a male 'who is
essentially a "sperm donor" to the homosexual
pair'. Both females will lay eggs in 'supernormal
clutches'. Sometimes parents bond homosexually,
and sometimes offspring are raised by single
females [with another lengthy bizarre list]. There
can be egg donors as well. Young can also be
adopted. Apparently some cross species with birds.
Sometimes same-sex pairs kidnap nests from
heterosexual pairs or steal eggs.
There are apparently 'no significant differences
in quality of care' (25) and success in raising
young by same-sex pairs can even exceed
heterosexual ones [because dominant males can get
better conditions, at least with black swans].
Inferior conditions can be compensated by more
parental effort [same sex must never be inferior]
. Sometimes a nest is built together, often in the
same way as heterosexual pairs, but sometimes with
variations – "twin" or "joint" nests', larger
nests. Sometimes young are raised by mixed sex
polygamist groups 'where their mothers may have
lesbian interactions with each other' (26. There
are other family constellations including three
mothers or for animals, and single parents.
Homosexual behaviour, especially in geese, display
'some of the major patterns of male and female
homosexuality and the range of variation found
throughout the rest of the animal world'. With
Canada geese, male and females participate equally
in homosexual activity, but there are some 'gender
differences' in certain behaviours.
Characteristically females tend to participate
more in same-sex pairings, females form longer
lasting pair bonds, in some geese homosexual
activity is exclusively male, but overall 'male
homosexuality is slightly more prevalent' (27).
However this may indicate 'the general male
bias of many biological studies'. Different
species display different frequencies of
copulation. There may be 'gender differences…
Apparent in various behaviour types' (28) although
again maybe gender bias.
There are variations in the combinations of
different activities. Mounting appears to be
ubiquitous, oral sex is equally prevalent, group
sexual activities more common in males, actual
penetration may be more common in male
homosexuals. Sexual positions may vary.
Apparently, long lasting pair bonds are 'generally
not more characteristic of females' (29) [except
in the specific cases mentioned earlier?]
'Non-monogamy and divorce' are found
equally between male and female couples. Japanese
macaque apparently offer particular variants
across a range of behaviours, showing that 'the
complex intersection of factors is involved in the
expression of homosexuality in each gender'. We
must revise our preconceived ideas, and resist
stereotypes. Some sets of behaviours 'defy any
simplistic categorisation' (30) [but no hint the
need for something performative, at least as yet].
There is a beautiful formula to calculate the
frequency of homosexual behaviour in the gull
population, and careful records taken of
frequencies of things like homosexual mounts
between females among Kob antelopes [reminds me an
awful lot of Masters and Johnson]. We can
determine the rates of homosexuality — 'the number
of homosexual acts performed during a given period
of time', and 8 female Kob antelopes were studied
for 67 hours and then compared to 36 male
longtailed Hermit hummingbirds [positivism gone
absolutely fucking nuts --Barad cannot approve
of this surely?]. Of course we will need to know
what might be the normal rate of sexual activity,
so we should really compare homosexual with
heterosexual acts and see homosexual activity 'as
a proportion of all sexual activity' (31). When we
do this with Kob antelopes, we find that there's
quite a high rate of sexual activity with
homosexual mounts at 'only 9% of all sexual
activity' whereas with hummingbirds it is 25%. We
need to explore the factors involved before we can
generalise [we already have].
In terms of the total number of species, it is
'over 450' that have documented same-sex behaviour
[of the activities defined above], although 'over
1 million species are known to exist'. For mammals
and birds, the figures are 300 in a total of
13,000. So far, however only a few species have
been studied and we need detailed study,
perhaps 'at least 1000 hours of field observation'
before we can describe unusual but important
activities. We don't know 'how many species have
been studied to this depth' — perhaps only one or
2000. That would still leave '15 to 30% of animal
species exhibiting homosexual behaviour'. The
percentage may be higher because it is easy to
miss common behaviours [which might include
exclusively heterosexual behaviour] — for example
in nocturnal or elusive species, and there are
also 'problems in identifying individual animals'.
For example in some species, heterosexual mating
'has never been seen', or seen only rarely, in one
case only once. Cheetahs are particularly shy and
they have only been observed copulating in the
wild five times. New findings appear all the time,
including some of those described here, and much
was unknown until the late 1990s. The point is
that if we insisted on empirical verification, we
would 'have to conclude that many species never
engage in heterosexuality' either. In some cases,
we know more about homo than hetero [observer
bias here?] : hetero might be more difficult
to observe.
It is quite wrong to see homosexuality as rare, or
even as regular, especially 'without any numerical
or contextual information' (33) and we also need a
common standard of measurement and agreed
points of references. At the moment there are
examples of various 'tallies of particular
behaviours'. Sometimes innovative techniques have
had to be developed — for example noticing that
supernormal clutches of eggs indicate lesbian
activity [the great equation is based on that
finding.]. Injuries might indicate mating.
There are still subjective commentaries on
the same sort of quantitative data — one zoologist
thought that 7% homosexual copulations indicated
that this was common, while another insisted the
same finding showed it was rare. We also need an
activity or time budget to assess the proportion
of activity or time — because some species
interact infrequently with everybody. Overall, the
population engaged in homosexuality varies widely.
For all these reasons 'it is not always a
straightforward matter to calculate various
measures of frequency such as these', and
three particularly problematic species are
discussed.
'Observational methodologies' might also have
an effect (35). However, despite all the
difficulties it is possible to generalise about
homosexual activity at least in three categories —
'courtship, sexual, and pair bonding'. So on
average 25% of individuals engage in these
activities, ranging from 2 to 3% to entire troops
[of Bonabo]. About 25% of courtship activity
specifically is homosexual, and about the same for
sexual activity, ranging from 1 to 94%. Same-sex
pair bonding accounts for an average of 14% of all
pairs. Combining all together gives an overall
figure of 20% homosexual mammal and bird
activities, and this is taken to be perhaps the
best number. It 'collapses the multiplicity of
behaviours… Represents only a fraction of
the animals which homosexuality is been
documented… Glosses over many observational
and theoretical uncertainties… Misleadingly
equates often radically unlike phenomena' (36) so
we might need to abandon the whole attempt and
acknowledge that there is no single formula. 'In
the end though we must acknowledge that our
measures are at best imperfect — and what we
are attempting to quantify is, in many senses,
incalculable'.
We can challenge the conventional view that there
are two sexes. There is 'considerably richer'
arrangement than that. Sometimes females can
become males, sometimes there are no males,
sometimes there are males that are both male and
female, or mails that resemble males, and same-sex
activity, a 'a wide variety of alternative
sexualities and genders', even 'transvestism or
transsexuality'. There may be hermaphroditism,
parthogenic species, crossing of existing gender
categories, imitating the opposite sex or
physically becoming the opposite sex, or combining
characteristics of both sexes. Hermaphrodite does
not cover all the possibilities, although there
are such species which have both sets of
reproductive organs. There need to be two distinct
sexes first to get 'proper' homosexuality [note
the current controversies unaddressed here — 'in transsexuality,
individuals actually become the opposite sex…
Transvestism… 'Can involve almost total physical
resemblance between males and females or mimicry
of only certain… Characteristics' (38). Activities
can involve growing parts of the body that help
imitation, or adopting typical behaviours. Not all
homosexuality is transvestism, for example even
though it might feature. [Note that there might
appear to be something akin to rape at least among
Bighorn Sheep, because females 'do not permit
themselves to be mounted by males except when they
are in heat'(39) — and so do homosexual
males].
Sex change is another 'routine aspect of many
animals' lives', again with different variations
requiring different terminology according to who
changes into what and how. It can be quite
complex. As a result 'striped parrotfish have…
Five distinct genders' but only two
biological sexes (41). Particular patterns of
mating and social organisation that accompanies it
may be complex as well. Some fish can be both
transsexual and transvestite again producing 'an
even more complex gender system', and some fish
begin as one sex and turn into the other — and
sometimes back the other way.
What this all shows is that animals actually have
'incredibly sophisticated and complex systems of
social organisation and behavioural patterning in
many species' (42), far from a simple interaction
between two sexes. There is animal homosexuality
and it is 'a rich and multifaceted phenomenon that
is at least as complex and varied as
heterosexuality'. Sometimes there are special
behaviour patterns. Homosexual animals can be
'both affectionate and sexual towards one another'
and use different techniques from kissing to anal
intercourse. Pair bonds can also vary in many
ways, and young can be raised in all sorts of
different family configurations. Overall, 'the
lives of "queer" animals are far more diverse than
we could ever have imagined'.
Chapter 2
Lots of animals appear to court and seek out each
companions from the same-sex. Zoologists drift
into anthropomorphism by giving them names
or human qualities, with the intention of
reminding us of individuality. In fact 'there are
a number of genuine connections and points of
correspondence between animal and human
homosexuality as well as significant
differences' (44). There are problems in
extrapolating from one to the other. Some
people think that homosexuality is uniquely human.
Primate homosexuality can offer insights though.
'Overall, a cautionary note must be sounded'
because there is richness and complexity in both
animals and human.
There is a variety of forms of human homosexuality
as we know, and the forms are shaped by social and
historical contexts [for example ancient Greeks].
There is a counterpart for nearly every form in
animals. However, there is a general 'lack of an
adequate understanding and classification of
different types of homosexuality', and many
different animal behaviours have been seen as
similar to specific human ones — 'analogies'.
[Barad would reject these of course]. In each
case, there is often 'the conflation of many
different behaviour variables and diverse
patterns' with animals and humans. We also need to
consider 'consensuality, age, gender presentations
of partners' (45) to generate even more complexity
Homosexuality can be seen as behaviour arranged on
one axis representing gender-based and role-based
interaction, another referring to age
relationships, another to sexual orientation of
participants, and another to do with the social
status of the activity (whether sanctioned or
condemned). Using these 'typological axes' [for
our purposes] we can show a comparable variability
between humans and animals. Culture alone does not
explain the diversity of human homosexuality,
since a capacity for '"sexual plasticity"' may
be biological and shared with other species.
Animal cultures might be seen in terms of anything
that exceeds genetic programming — individual
habit, learned behaviour, community
'"traditions"'.
Generally speaking, most human activities have an
analogue among animals — laughing, killing, female
orgasm and so on. The specifics of homosexual
interactions might be different — for example
people can engage in exclusive homosexuality, have
more varied sexual motivations, and react with
hostility to sexual deviants [a crucial difference
-- see above] . Generally, we are still learning
about animals, but we know enough to suspect human
uniqueness and exceptionalism.
Take exclusive or preferred homosexuality. There
may be exclusive homosexuality of various types in
'more than 60 species of nondomestic mammals and
birds' (47). It is complex, because exclusivity
itself may vary in terms of time and
appropriateness. Long-term exclusivity is the most
contested, and here, there are difficulties
because there are differences in life expectancy.
The operational definition is 'homosexual activity
that continues for more than two consecutive
years. It could only be absolutely verified by
tracking a large number of individuals over their
lives and this is obviously difficult — the same
problem affects exclusive heterosexuality. Studies
of gulls, geese and penguins come closest and
there is some evidence of continuous pair bonds
lasting as long as 15 years, until death [although
there can also be '"divorce"']. Other birds
suggest similar patterns. Often there is no
heterosexual activity. There may be a connection
with whether heterosexual relationships are also
lifelong. Exclusively homosexual long-term pairing
is rarer in mammals, although bottlenose dolphins
show it. 'It is quite likely' that these are
exclusively homosexual, although it is very
difficult to monitor sexual behaviour in all
conditions. Generally, lifetime bonding is no more
prominent in heterosexual than in homosexual
couples. There are significant portions of the
population that do not breed or do hetero sex.
There are sometimes sex segregated groups with
variable status [usually male only bands].
Sometimes only dominant males mate with females.
Sometimes sexual relations are also incestuous.
Overall 'exclusivity can be inferred' from
patterns including nonbreeding.
There can be homosexual friendships, and
bisexuality — occurring 'in more than half of the
mammal and bird species in which same-sex activity
is found' (50). There are many forms and degrees
of bisexuality, including sequential versus
simultaneous, sometimes affected by seasons, and
sometimes alternating with either homo or hetero
sexuality. There can be copulation in threesomes,
or quartets [massive detail here again]. There can
even be individual differences, 'a unique sexual
orientation profile' (51). We can use the same
scale as Kinsey [!] [irredemably
positivist] used to discuss human
sexual orientation [which is highly controversial
of course], especially for Bonobo — another
argument that there is nothing uniquely human in
sexuality. [One study discussed in slightly more
detail consists of observations of a troupe of 10
female Bonobo. The redoubtable zoologist
'tabulated all their homosexual genital rubbings
[classified how -- how were they different from
other kinds of genital rubbings -- status
enforcement, say] versus heterosexual copulations'
over three months]. There seems to be a spectrum
in sexual behaviour.
Preference is obviously 'a rather elusive
concept to measure', and 'identity' is even
more slippery. We have to work with clues to
preferences — for example choice of homosexual
activity despite the availability of members of
the other sex, competition for sexual attention
[which indicates motivation], refusals of sexual
contacts, continuing sexual preference even after
the loss of a mate. Again this happens with 'more
than 50 mammals and birds' (53). Some species
indicate clear heterosexual references, with
perhaps only a '" latent" bisexuality.
Consensuality is important. There may be
duress or rape, just as with heterosexual
relations. Others 'willingly [?] mate with animals
of both sexes'. The frequency of bisexual and
homosexual activity varies across the species,
from near universal to rare. They may be combined
[in sexual careers]. Generally there are multiple
dimensions, but overall, bisexuality seems more
common than homosexuality. There is enough
evidence to suggest that this is 'not a uniquely
human phenomenon'. Universal heterosexuality 'is
also certainly less than ubiquitous'. Overall,
multiple shades of sexual orientation are found
throughout the animal world' (54).
We might try to study the status of individuals
with different sexual orientations in their own
communities. Spatial relations might be a clue
here, or hostility and segregation. There are some
suggestions that this is not apparent for
homosexual individuals who 'are completely
integrated [have no position?] into the social
fabric of the species', 'greeted with
nonchalance', with individuals moving effortlessly
between different forms [including dominant
ones?]. There may be 'simple curiosity', or wish
to participate — and group sessions can develop,
in both homo and hetero. Bisexuality seems to
particularly prevent hostility between homo and
hetero — and multiple participations are possible.
Generally, homosexual activity is 'regarded [!] as
routinely as heterosexual activity' (55). If
anything, heterosexual copulations are more
'regularly harassed and interrupted' [there may be
an age dimension it seems, or an effect of
breeding]. Some homosexual partners have become
powerful members, perhaps because they draw on the
combined strength of paired males. Home
territories are defended with combined male
vigour. Some homosexual and transgendered animals
can even terrorise others [black swans again],
sometimes combined with sexual jealousy of one of
the pair. Territory may be invaded. Homosexual
partners may be more aggressive — often
heterosexual individuals being attacked [for some
other reason than just their 'preferences'?]
There are some cases of homosexual animals being
targeted, usually males interfering between two
females in order to gain access to one of them,
but this rarely prevents variations. Transgendered
deer [sic -- they have different antlers,
apparently] might be ostracised and have to form
their own groups, but this is rare, and segregation
is often based on other factors like age,
social rank, physical disability and so on. [but
not acceptance?] It may be that male-female pairs
can defend their nest sites better than female
female ones. Individuals may pursue their own
segregation and isolation [— try this for anthropomorphism:
'female Japanese macaques in homosexual consort
ships… Isolate themselves physically and socially
from other troop members… To spend time together'
(58)]. Of course 'multiple factors were
undoubtedly involved, as is true for the formation
of segregated groups of homosexuals among people'
(59), as in gay ghettos or subcultures [same as?]
, which can even take on positive factors [in
both?].
Generally, adverse responses to homosexuality is
'uncharacteristic of animal societies' [but he
can't allow it to be unique to human ones]. They
are just part of animal social life. Animals are
often more humane than humans, and 'might even
offer us models' for how to tolerate differently
oriented individuals.
Overall it looks as if at least three species
'rival, if not equal, human beings and the
variability and "completeness" of the sexual
expression: Bpnabo, orang utan and bottle nosed
dolphins'. Each [deviant] variant is matched by a
same-sex option. There are other
stratifications based on age and social status,
so there are patterns — younger female ones are
more likely to do same-sex, and distance between
the ranks helps [no suggestion of hierarchy or
duress]. [A lot of comparisons are suggested in
descriptions here, including the comparability
between animals and humans in preferences for 'a
face-to-face position' (60), and an ability to
engage in sex throughout the female cycle. Animals
also do anal and oral sex] [the different
factors are never weighted in terms of
importance, but sexuality can be assumed to be
dominant?
Sexual technique also varies according to
long-term relationships in humans and animals —
there can be 'sexual "virtuosos"' in both, more
frequently in same-sex partners, 'availability of
this claim with respect to humans cannot be
directly addressed here' — he claims we can get
more accuracy with animals, because there are
wider sexual repertoires in homosexual
activities]. There are variations in sexual
virtuosity between homo and hetero in different
species, however, and some mixed cases where
heteros engage in 'anal or rump stimulation '(61).
Most encounters either hetero or homo are
'"uninspired"', however [values
here!!] Versatility itself is difficult
to define — most mounting versatility
specifically is found in heteros. Particular
species might enjoy a particular variety — seven
different mounting positions have been identified
among Japanese Macaques [hilarious positivism
again], and heteros can show more flexibility in
terms of positions commonly used [hilarious and
elaborate examples covering dolphins, parrots,
hummingbirds, plovers and woodpeckers]. Courting
behaviour similarly varies.
On to humans. Animal studies show that 'homosexuality
is part of our evolutionary heritage as primates'(64)
and may date back to the Oligocene epoch. Other
primates show considerable sexual [deviance]. We
also have cultural heritage, but this also
belongs to our status as primates.
[evolutionary psychology explains culture].There
are 'startling examples of cultural traditions
among animals' (65) — 'many animals innovate
behaviours and then pass them on', and this is culture
for zoologists, or at least proto-culture.
We can see these effects in a wide range of
behaviours, from hunting techniques to song
dialects and migration patterns. Japanese macaques
come to the fore again in passing on learned
behaviours. Other signs are particular patterns
found only in particular populations. Sexual
behaviour itself shows such variations, with
particular types becoming more popular in some
troops than others, and changing over time. So
capacity may be biological, but occurrence is
variable enough to be considered as cultural [in
animals --but the other way around for humans]
Cultural sexuality fits with other cultural
innovations for example non-reproductive sexual
activities and their role in the development of
cultural milestones — communication systems and
the origin of language. Of course 'caution
must always be exercised in making direct
comparisons between animals and people, and
most these areas are only beginning to be studied
in any detail' (66) [much of it seems to turn on
evolutionary psychology?]
[This next bit is quite good on
the role of sex in developing communication and
tool {sic} use]
Bonobo seem to have a system of gestural
communication used during sex, for example, and
further language uses may have developed based on
hand and arm gestures [described in hilarious
detail]. This assumes that these hand signals are
'iconic', resembling particular body movements
which are intended, but they are also signs of
conventional organisation and the need for mutual
understanding and learning. There are also
positioning movements involving direct touching [
a lexicon appears on page 67]. More abstract
signals may have developed, development in coding
'identified in the development of human sign
languages' (68). Bonobo not have a 'complete human
linguistic system', but they have a certain level
of sophistication, perhaps even 'a rudimentary
"syntax"' and it may be a precursor to
human language. Some [evolutionary] linguists
suggest that human communication began with
gestures, perhaps as a response to the need to
coordinate hunting
The animals learn to develop this on their own.
Attempts to teach sign language have also
demonstrated the possession of 'formidable
linguistic capacities' following human prompting.
For Bonobo, sexual activity seems to have been
'the specific social context that prompted this
development' because it is so plastic and
variable. This suggests that sexual context
might have been important in human language as
well.
People think that the use of tools is unique to
humans, but again we know that inanimate objects
are used to manipulate things in the environment
and these 'can be seen as precursors to similar
activities in human beings' (69). Lots of examples
follow — chimpanzees with twigs, macaques grooming
each other with sticks. Some of them are used in
sexual stimulation, such as 'masturbatory aids',
and this has been a bit neglected. Objects can be
rubbed or inserted, penises inserted into openings
in leaves or fruit, making leaves vibrate, using
sticks in bundles as dildoes, sometimes after
modifications, 'often in highly creative ways'
(71), similar to activities used in people. We
might even conclude that 'sexual stimulation may
have been a component of tool use among early
humans'. There are utilitarian functions as well.
Most human cultures have taboos surrounding sexual
relations, such as the incest taboo, and these
have 'clearly strong social and cultural
components'. There is some variation among humans,
however, even here. The cultural dimension is also
found in 'the role played by social familiarity as
opposed to genetic relatedness in choice of
partners' (72). Homosexual relations can also be
prohibited between related individuals,
'nonbiological factors in the incest taboo', even
when there is more tolerance of same-sex
relations. So biological matters such as birth
defects from inbreeding are not adequate — apart
from anything else inbreeding has taken place in
some small populations without adverse effects.
What of incest taboos in animals? There are some
similarities — 'many animal species actually show
evidence of a "cultural" or "social" dimension to
their avoidance of sexual activity between
relatives' [I'd like to see the evidence for
that]. There is certainly a lot of
diversity. In some prime groups they do seem to be
taboos — because 'incest of any sort is not
common' (73). It might vary between hetero and
homo. Some species have apparently defined a
notion of socially acceptable partner. [Japanese
bleeding macaques again] [evidence for this is the
frequency of particular kinds of sexual relations
between kin animals, leading to hilarious
positivism again such as 'more than a quarter of
all mounts between females occur between half
sisters']. 'These choices are not due to instinct…
Because no offspring result', and taboo is are not
necessarily identical. Some seem to be specific to
homosexual activity, which might be considered to
be less affected by instinct or genetics.
Human rituals are quite important, but they might
be based on sexual relations — a couple of
primatologists say that the homo sexualised
activities between male baboons have been
important as models in human symbolism [but
no other baboon behaviours] — the baboons do
sexual and affectionate behaviours, and may be
'"greetings" interactions… [serving to]… negotiate
and solidify cooperation between males', by
risking their sexual organs in the hands of
others. , Humans do not fondle genitals,but the
same loonies think these are similar gestures —
holding the penis is found in Australian
aboriginal tribes [and Janners]. They are even
found in Judaeo-Christian ceremonies — references
to placing the hand on the Masters loins, or
indicated by the connection between 'testify,
testimony, and testicle' (75). These connections
'are somewhat speculative' [and seemingly based on
the work of two particular loonies]. The
differences are also 'vast'— but the forms might
be similar.
Overall, cultural development in the form of
language, tools, taboo, and ritual might be 'much
more intimately associated' with primate
behaviours than was thought, and sexual versions
may have been important in cultural development.
Is often thought that homosexuality does not occur
in nature and therefore that human versions are
unnatural. But this whole concept is too simple.
For one thing, lots of human activity does not
occur in nature but is not condemned, and some
animal behaviours including 'diseases, birth
defects, rape, and cannibalism' (77) are not
considered desirable. Historical attitudes towards
homosexuality have varied independently of how
natural or unnatural it seems — sometimes it's
unnatural nature was 'used to justify its
superiority to heterosexuality. In ancient
Greece'. Homosexuality is seen as unhealthily
close to nature, subhuman. Biological bases have
varied in terms of their role in social
discrimination — 'racial' differences might be
natural, but this has not prevented
discrimination.
More generally, the binaries or dichotomies seem
inadequate, and complex interactions are more
profitable — both environment and biology in
people and animals. Of course heterosexual
relations are seen to be non-deviant, but not all
sexual activity fits into exclusive categories.
The discussion about animal homosexuality is
rarely invoked. Complexity again questions simple
origins and binaries. If anything, 'homosexual
behaviour is as natural as heterosexual
behaviour', widely found in a variety of forms.
And, conversely, heterosexual activity is far from
being simply natural, and frequently 'exhibits
social elaboration or cultural "embellishment"'
(79), including forms of acceptability,.
Animals usually play a symbolic role in human
affairs, in ways that have 'little to do with
their biological and social realities' [really?
totemism?], although interpretations placed upon
their behaviours can have grave consequences if
moral values are implied [including for animals —
some are seen as having desirable family
practices]. It is far less easy to consider some
species as deserving if they actually know about
the variety of sexual activity that they display —
we are far from simple family values.
Homosexuality in particular is not uniform, has
many forms, is the result of biology and
environment. Even separating culture from biology
is complex and simple dichotomies should be
avoided.
Pets and domesticated animals also display
variety, but are likely to be invested with
greater levels of interpretation and emotion. Most
pet owners will be aware of same-sex activity, and
there are scientific studies which confirm it,
even 'ejaculation during interaction between
males' in dogs and cats. Homosexual behaviour is
widely apparent, even 'routine among domesticated
mammals' (81), often attracting special vernacular
terminology. The wild relatives of these animals
display the same variety. Homosexuality in pets
and other domesticated animals is 'scientifically
verified' (82), but there is still a great deal of
variety across species, and many meanings are
available to people who work with them — sometimes
homosexual behaviour in farm animals, for example,
is deplored, and sometimes welcomed.
[Chapter 3 looks at the history of understandings
of animal homosexuality. Chapter 4 looks at ways
of explaining away animal homosexuality. Chapter 5
shows some of the possible evolutionary values of
homosexuality and alternative sexualities,
possibly expanding some of the ones we've got
already. I have shifted to chapter 6]
Chapter 6
Science has grappled with animal homosexuality and
has been unsuccessful in seeing it as just a
variant of reproductive behaviour. As a result we
need to rethink some of our concepts. We might
turn here to 'the traditional knowledge of
indigenous and tribal cultures… Aboriginal
worldviews' (214) which see gender and sexuality
as 'inherently multiple and mutable'. Indigenous
beliefs show 'remarkable correspondences with
recent scientific discoveries in animal behaviour…
Perspectives such as chaos science, post-Darwinian
evolution, Gaia theory, biodiversity studies, and
the theory of General Economy' (215). His
preferred approach is 'the concept of Biological
Exuberance' which will synthesise indigenous and
modern views. It sets out to oppose the usual view
that scarcity and functionality of driven
biological changes — abundance and excess have had
an equal role, and as a result diverse sexual
activity is an expected outcome, a form of
biological extravagance.
Homosexuality ceases to be an anomaly that
requires explanation. Many indigenous cultures see
it as 'routine and expected'. Those cultures have
already accumulated a storehouse of knowledge
about the natural world, including the sexuality
of animals 'over thousands of years', and so
science might be able to learn something.
A suitably complex understanding is found in
native North America, Melanesia, and
Siberian/Arctic peoples, and these have been
particularly well studied. Although beliefs
actually vary, forms are similar. They are
understood as either 'totemic or symbolic
associations of animals with human homosexuality…
Beliefs about mutable or non-dualistic genders…
Often represented in the figure of a powerful
cross gendered animal… Ceremonial re-enactments or
representations of animal [sexual variants,]
sometimes combined with ritual reversals… Animal
husbandry practices that encourage and value…
Nonreproductive creatures' (216). [So classic
Levi-Strauss stuff here -- animals seem
ambiguous in ways which include their sexual
behaviour, so they can play important mediating
roles -- cf the mediating importance of
ambiguous objects too like tobacco or honey.It
is the mediating roles which are important, not
the anomalies presented by bisexual animals]
In native America, homosexual and transgender
persons are seen as possessing two spirits which
is sacred. Such people are frequently shamans or
healers. They may have certain animals
symbolically associated with them, or favour
particular clans or creation stories [lots of
examples]. They may be tricksters, shape shifters,
mediators between humans and animals or men and
women, or ourselves and others. They may be
implicated in sacred quests or dreams. They may be
associated with birds that have striking colour
schemes of two colours [including magpies]. In
some places they are associated with left-handed
things including left-handed bears, and bears are
often seen 'as a powerful cross gendered figure'
(217), both with male aggression and ferocity, and
yet with female qualities such as life-giving.
Left-handedness is 'traditionally associated with
the feminine' such cultures so bears are seen to
be left-handed as well, and there may be taboos
about using right-hands to hunt them. There is
even '"left-handed speech"' (218) where
'special affixes can be added to words' [so is
left-handedness the main thing or sexual
ambiguity?]. Male coyotes may be tricksters
[Denzin would be very happy], and they may marry
different animals or be homosexual or transgender
[to trick other animals into marriage] [so again
is tricking the central thing with sexual tricking
a part of it?]
There are 'continuing' links between animals and
homosexuality or transgender [some examples of
folktales about coyote follow]. Sometimes
ambiguous animals are used in human rituals — as
well as 'shamanic rights of self mutilation… Feats
of astounding physical endurance, and graphic
sexual imagery' (219). A particular dance
'culminates on the final day with symbolic
homosexual activity between the bison bulls' and
another character with a wooden penis. There are
rather ceremonial performances in various animal
rites [interesting examples e.g. 220 F]. There may
be reversed activities or ritual killing,
including 'ritual transgender'[which seems to be
humans adopting oath male and female animal
attributes]. Humans can imitate homosexual
couplings of animals, or animal birth, and this
can carry over into animal husbandry which
acknowledges gender and sexual variability —
hermaphrodite sheep and goats might be thought to
be particularly valuable, for example.
In New Guinea homosexuality and transgender 'also
feature prominently among the indigenous peoples'.
Homosexuality is part of social and ceremonial
interaction and [deviant] animals are equally
pervasive. Sometimes 'all males undergo a period
of homosexual initiation'. Sometimes semen
masculinises boys [this has also been condemned as
a form of child abuse]. Some groups recognise a
third sex — hermaphrodites or intersexed people.
Homosexuality can featuring creation myths, in
male parthenogenesis. Animals can also be
'symbolically associated with homosexuality'(222).
Some animals are thought to have sexual [careers],
sometimes involving 'a daily oscillation between
genders' [and we should take this seriously as
indigenous knowledge?]. Some dance ceremonies
mimic animal courtship, sometimes with gender
inversion. The cassowary in particular is
considered to be androgynous or gender mixing,
combining masculine and feminine attributes,
sometimes seen as androgynous or hermaphrodite,
undergoing transformation. It can be particularly
valued where 'ritualised homosexuality" is
practised (223). [There seems to be quite a bit of
biological confusion here too — that the cassowary
has a penis, gives birth through the anus, suckle
their young from neck wattles -- but see below].
These features can emerge in human rituals
involving cassowary dancers with both male and
female characteristics. The cassowary can rule
over a whole group of 'androgynous and sex
transforming animals' in one particular 'remote
tribe" (224), represented by various divine
creator figures, and represented by special humans
in rituals and ceremonies: in one 'they are
sometimes referred to as "male mothers"' and can
cross dress. Sometimes they are 'physically
intersexual or hermaphrodite members of the
tribe'. They are associated with the life force
itself.
Other groups in Vanuatu perform homosexuality in
various ways [sometimes physically and sometimes
symbolically] in secret ceremonies, all of which
involve 'the image of the shark' (225). Sometimes
pigs are seen to be androgynous at least when they
first appeared, and actual hermaphrodites are
highly prized and encouraged so that nearly every
village has some. Sometimes they are attributed
'seven distinct "genders' (226), a particularly
fruitful 'indigenous classification' which
'exceeds in completeness any conceptual or
non-nomenclatural system developed by Western
science' [usual cringing stuff]. Men who raise
hermaphrodite pics might be seen as sexually
ambiguous themselves.
In Siberia/Arctic there is 'a similar
constellation of phenomena'. There are cross
gender animal spirit guides, shamans who can
assume characteristics of the other sex, 'gender
reversals and re-combinations' where there are
[sexual careers]. They may be associated with
particular animals and feature in many
myths. There may be animal impersonation combined
with cross dressing, again often 'aimed at
promoting sexual activity and a "renewal of life"'
(227). Some dances imitate animal courtship
displays. Reindeer might be seen as particularly
powerful transgendered creatures ['and are thought
to hatch from giant eggs on the tundra']. Clothing
associated with these rituals might also display
mixed characteristics.
Some Inuit recognise a third gender or sex, and
are familiar with transsexuals and transvestites
and mythically transformed shamans. An originating
deity bridges opposites including male and female,
animal and human. Again there is are sometimes
seen as mixed gender, and this is revealed in
various ceremonies involving beavers. There can be
'bawdy displays of transvestism', simulated
homosexual activity, various other transgressions
and reversals. [Lots of fascinating examples again
for example 229]. Spirit helpers may often be seen
as hermaphrodite and impersonated by cross
dressing humans or ambiguous animals.
Nonbreeding animals can also feature in some
husbandry practices, although they are not
apparently given special meaning. This includes
gelded or barren animals, who are considered
essential for the welfare of the whole herd,
embodying 'cosmic life force' and fertility (230).
There are 'wide differences in cultural context
and details' but also 'a number of remarkable
correspondences and continuities' between these
three indigenous groups. There may indeed be five
central themes: animals are symbolically
associated with homosexuality or transgender;
gender-mixing creatures can be particularly
important in cosmology; ritual enactments of
animal homosexuality and transgender are
commonplace and are associated with fertility or
life; there might be a male mother figure, or
patterns of reversals or inversions. Hermaphrodite
and nonbreeding animals are cultivated and might
be highly valued, and ambiguous animals and people
'are consistently honoured and ceremonialised'. A
continuity is seen between human and nonhuman
creatures. There may be 'implications for
contemporary scientific thought'.
These views may not be literally accurate, and
'many of the more "fanciful" indigenous beliefs
about animals are obviously false (at least in
their specifics)' (231). But there may be some
useful observed connections. For example
homosexuality is common among bison magpies or
bears, or in closely related animals — emus and
ostriches if not exactly for cassowaries [but this
course assumes that indigenous peoples know about
these other animals]. Correspondences of this kind
are summarised in the table, pages 231 – 33
[another paratactic device that offers no chance
for intervention].
Transgender seems to offer more precise
correspondences, especially into sexuality. Modern
science 'has provided startling confirmation of a
number of indigenous "beliefs"' about cross
gendered animals — 'some species of bears
probably are left paw dominant'[pretty weak
confirmation!]. Left-handedness might be
common, and there is not always consistency, but
polar bears seem to be consistently left-handed [a
brilliant study showed that they were more
commonly trapped by extending their left limbs
into the trap]. There 'also appears to be a
correlation between left-handedness and
homosexuality/transgender in humans' (234) [must
look up the reference]. Bears are indeed attracted
to human menstrual blood as in the myths,
discovered by zoologists 'employing controlled
olfactory – preference tests'. There may even be
'actual cases of physical gender mixing in bears',
and one study found 'significant numbers of
"masculinised females"' in wild populations,
perhaps between 10 and 20% 'in some populations.
They can have different sorts of genitalia, they
can combine the phallus and the vagina 'so the
female actually mates and gives birth through the
tip of her "penis"'.
Combinations of male and female sex organs can
also be found in other animals even though they
are 'not usually hermaphroditic' (235). There is
so much profusion that scientists of that to
develop new terminology, sometimes drawing on
Greek mythology such as chimera or mosaic [the
implications they should use indigenous peoples
names]. There can be all sorts of chromosome
configurations such as 'XXY, XXX, XXXY, XO' and
this will produce mixtures of sex organs and
sexual characteristics. One animal is actually
'literary divided in half' (236) and is called a
gynandromorph. [And are these sexually ambiguous
in activity?]
'Recent discoveries by zoologists' are remarkably
similar to indigenous ideas, for example that
would lemmings are indeed often female and
chromosomally male., Or that moles can do sex
reversal, even primates can. Female caribou may
not be mixed gender, but they can 'exhibit
physical "transvestism" in the sense that they
bear antlers'. There are similar parallels with
the beliefs about birds who can reverse roles,
sometimes associate in homosexual pairs, or
display a 'polyandrous social system' in the case
of cassowaries. Those birds do have unusual
anatomies — males can possess a kind of penis
which is also '"invaginated"' (237) and they have
cloacas rather than anuses. 'Most amazingly,
all-female cassowaries also possess a phallus',
similar to the males but smaller — 'a "male
clitoris"'.
There may be similarities with the notion of
hyper- masculinity, excessive masculinity
displayed sometimes in two -spirit characters.
This is sometimes associated with homosexuality as
masculinising men. We find this in birds as well
as other animals.
'Are the various connections between indigenous
beliefs and scientific facts merely fortuitous, or
do they represent accurate observation of animals
on the part of aboriginal cultures?' (238). They
do depend on some 'often esoteric details of
animal behaviour', itself grounded in direct
observation and study, even though 'encoded in
mythological terms'. Aboriginal knowledge 'often
mirrors the findings of more "objective"
scientific enquiry, sometimes down to the most
minute detail'. Indigenous classification systems
are sometimes more comprehensive than scientific
ones. Some of the qualities known have only just
recently been discovered by science. Sometimes
indigenous knowledge exceeds scientific
understanding, for example in the behaviour of
walrus among the Inuit, or the habits of beavers
among the Cree.
So there might be particularly accurate
understanding of gender mixing. The variety of
useful names for these creatures is significant.
First-hand observations seems at least as
important as mythology. Sometimes it can exceed
Western science. Left-handedness in bears is an
example of accurate observation rather than
figments of the imagination [yes --but the
connection with sexuality is imagination?].
Widespread animal homosexuality and transgender
might also indicate that indigenous cultures knew
more about these activities than Western science
do, and that designations of homosexuality might
be in advance of Western science, indicated 'time
and again' [one example is Navajo discovery of a
bird before scientists discovered it — and lots of
others]. The discovery of indigenous
pharmaceuticals offers a similar story, as do
reports of self-medicating behaviour in animals —
there is now a whole new 'exciting field of
enquiry' called zoopharmacognosy (241).
So oral traditions are often not as mistaken as we
claim, but should be seen instead as 'repositories
of a scientific tradition' (242). They should not
be just dismissed as inaccurate, because science
itself misses many examples or ignores those where
there is a 'strong personal distaste for the
subject matter' as in homosexual behaviour. We
should see them as signposts for future research,
suggesting birds to study or behaviours to
investigate. We should form a partnership [?]
between the two paradigms, and there have
been some useful collaborations [detailed on 243].
There is a course of particular implication for
homosexuality and transgender characteristics. The
value of indigenous beliefs is not so much the
accuracy but 'the overall worldview imparted by
these cultures' (243) [that there are multiple
possibilities]. If the possibilities are
recognised in animals, then they tend to be better
accepted among humans. Only silly Westerners find
it impossible to imagine queer animals. While
Westerners see homosexuality as an insult related
to purity or virility 'sentiments, less overtly
throughout the scientific discourse' (244),
natives see homosexuality 'as an affirmation of
nature's plurality, strength, and wholeness' and
so can actually be the more accurate of the two
views. Inclusion is more important than literal
accuracy anyway [make your mind up].
Chaos theory is now fashionable and we should
embrace it, together with '"perhaps a more
mystical concept of animals"' [quoting some
naturalists]. This would break away from narrowly
functional explanations and recognise 'inherent
multiplicity' (245). There are no narrow functions
— existing is the only purpose [not quite
performance then?]. These new insights comprise
Biological Exuberance, as a shift in perspective,
designed to find new patterns in existing facts.
Many ideas are still speculative and
controversial, but sometimes they turn out to be
compatible with orthodox theories after all [so
make your mind up again]. We do need a
breakthrough [interesting term], a paradigm shift.
There is a post-Darwinian version of evolution,
that sees nature as erratic, discontinuous, with
lots of contingency, and signs of 'the self
organisation of life' (246), that environments can
alter genetic codes, and that natural selection is
no longer 'hegemonic'. There is a convergence with
chaos theory. [Deleuze and Guattari have had a go
at this too, of course -- see DeLanda ]. This
might provide new insights into things like
extinction of species, mimicry between animals,
convergences between biological and inorganic
forms. There are implications for the other
sciences
Take self organisation of life. Assemblages do not
happen at random but can form '"spontaneously"'
[enabling a weasel over life forces?]. Underlying
processes may produce convergences in form and in
this way 'actually "direct" evolutionary change'.
There is also a notion of a '"fluid genome"'
where the environment changes genes, and there is
a two-way interaction which may produce new
species. All this is still early, but some famous
biologists are on board. For someone called Edward
O Wilson, for example, the conventional notion of
evolution is a myth, 'a form of religion'. The new
approaches are much more open [that 'good word'
that we encounter so often]. Particular attention
is focused on the role of random genetic
variations, which has long been challenged by
Stephen Jay Gould among others [he seems to have
challenged in particular the idea that every
surviving form reflects an adaptive mechanism].
'Bizarre' examples of sexuality might be
particularly relevant here.
There are parallels with chaos theory, applied
quite early to biology and the fluctuations of
populations, or to understand natural fractals.
According to one view, '"evolution is chaos with
feedback"' (247). Enthusiasts have searched
particularly for 'arrhythmias, discordant
harmonies, and aperiodicities' to complement the
search for pattern. Evolutionary adaptation has
been challenged on the grounds that first we need
to explain the 'incredible variety' that occurs in
the first place — for example in 'plumage
diversity', or in the diversity of sex and gender
that we have been examining. Some of this
behaviour seems to be counter-productive but also
normal, and we should attempt to explain
apparently aberrant behaviour as a part of the
system [classic deleuzian stuff here][I first came
across with RK Merton attempting to explain
juvenile delinquency as a normal but illegal
response to 'social strain'] . Complexity needs to
be examined, including that found in sexual
behaviours.
Goerner has offered five principles for chaos. The
main implications are: nonlinear systems may show
qualitative changes in behaviour; there may be
multiple competing forms of behaviour each of them
stable; alternative sexualities can therefore be
seen as 'alternative manifestations of a single
sexual "dynamic"', (248) with 'endless and
infinitely varying expressions'. Sexual behaviour
would be a good test for chaos theory. Overall,
seemingly incoherent phenomena may be part of a
wider pattern.
There is also Lovelock's Gaia theory, which has
had a substantial impact on science and evolution.
Everything forms 'a single self-regulating entity,
analogous to a giant living organism' (249)
[Sociologists have long had doubts about this] .
Cooperation is as important as competition,
adaptive explanations operate with species rather
than individuals, or the whole biosphere. A number
of innovative ideas have led to attempts at
verification. Implications for homosexuality are
also important if we see that reproduction is not
just prompted by a drive to survive — 'it may be
beneficial for a species or ecosystem as a whole
if some of its members do not procreate' (249).
Homosexuality is not the same as non-reproduction,
however, and there is 'little evidence' that
it helps to regulate populations [as a
latent function]. But it is paradoxical and mixed,
illustrating 'multiplicity within oneness'[but it
was argued before that it was not entirely
biological].
[Then arguing the case nevertheless] Sexual
diversity may have a role in a larger whole, as
with biodiversity generally, now taken as a
measure of the vitality of the system. Usually
this is sort of in physical terms, such as the
number of different species, but sexual diversity
also seems so common that it might be similar [the
example is the tremendous diversity apparently
among sandpipers — described at some length on
250]. Such diversity can lead to species success,
greater adaptability and flexibility. Even
homosexuality might be linked to 'environmental or
social changes' — for example when golden plovers
become more active homosexually when severe
winters disrupt heterosexual pairing [why should
this result? We seem to have some compensation
notion here that if golden plovers can't get
hetero sex, they are mightily relieved to get homo
sex]. Female co-parenting among grizzlies is more
common 'in conditions of environmental or social
flux' [equally puzzling], and ostriches do more
homosexual courtship in excessively rainy seasons.
'The correlations between these factors need to
be more systematically investigated' (251)
but at least we should not expect some linear
one-way cause-and-effect relationship [of course],
but rather plasticity. It might be plasticity that
enables helpful responses [but why sexual
plasticity?].
If we take sexual versatility as both biological
and cultural we can find more links to species
success — a certain G Gray Eaton suggest that
plastic behaviour means general adaptability, in
humans as well as among macaques. This is not
claiming an inevitable function, although there
may be specific functions, but rather 'a
manifestation of the larger "chaotic ordering" or
nonlinearity of the world'. Looking for
specific contributions might be too literal.
General flexibility might be the valuable thing.
So we need to examine sexual diversity as
biodiversity, but also including social
organisations. This then might produce 'an
essential measure of biological vitality' the more
diverse they become. Mating and courtship patterns
are important after all. Generally, many specific
purposes of behaviour are difficult to see anyway.
Thinking in terms of the 'overall complexity and
vitality of the environment' (252) might be more
productive rather than anything too narrowly
utilitarian. [Then it gets quite pious and
communitarian…] 'Every individual, every
behaviour — whether productive or
"counter-productive", comprising 1% or 99% of the
population — has a part to play'. 'Biological
diversity is intrinsically valuable, and
homosexuality/transgender is one reflection of
that diversity' [no doubt, but the argument
earlier was that it was a particularly valuable
component].
The common theme with the new approaches is that
natural systems display 'profound extravagance',
whether fractal systems, strange attractors, or
biological tropical exuberance including birdsong.
Birdsong in particular is too rich and varied for
simple functional explanations does this make it a
language as Guattari
seemed to think?] . Even insects displayed
diverse and complex structures, and some have
particularly '"apparently superfluous complexity"'
of their genitalia. Luxuriant features, whether
peacock tail feathers or fleshy dewlaps can now
make sense.
The idea is based on Bataille [!] In the notion of
'General Economy' (253). Excess exuberance are
driving forces of biological systems, more so than
scarcity or competition or narrow functionality.
Bataille's point is that all organisms have more
energy than they actually need to stay alive — it
is used to grow, but even then there is an excess
that must be used up in some way. Bataille thought
it was through sexual reproduction, eating, and
death [hunting?]. This superabundance of energy
comes from the sun, and it provides life with a
problem of how to manage excess. It leads to
excessive ornament or consumption, or elaboration,
or new species, in both humans and animals. So
life is actually full of extravagant and excessive
activities. In human life there are many attempts
to control such exuberance in religion or
political systems. [sounds a bit like deleuzian
'desire']
The whole approach 'accords startlingly well'
with some observations in science, including those
discussed above. Sometimes there are specific
confirmations too. Biologists have noted that
sexual reproduction is wasteful, and can even
damage individuals or whole populations [like
insects who die after mating]. It seems
inefficient compared to non-sexual reproduction,
'generally considered to be more than twice as
"expensive" (energetically as well as
genetically)' (254). Similar points can be made
about eating — after all plants manufacture their
own food. Death as well, such as the enormous
death rates of baby animals. Nature is not cruel
but abundant and excessive.
This approach is also 'intuitively accessible'
— we can see excess in the lush plants in our
garden []bred in by us] , varied snowflakes,
complex colourings of leaves. Bataille is right to
accord this great importance, rather than seeing
extravagance as the result of byproduct of
something like evolution. Above all we can
understand the phenomenon of homosexuality in this
light — it is classically '"wasteful"' (255), even
more so than heterosexual activity, and thus
has an important role in energy expenditure.
It is already the case that reproduction is
limited to only a few in many cases. There is both
'prodigious fecundity and fruitless prodigality'.
Exuberance is also stressed in nonscientific
indigenous worldviews and their cosmologies. EO
Wilson again has seen Amazonian shamans or people
in New Guinea as particularly keen on exuberance,
and some indigenous people seem to be into fractal
architecture. Indigenous cultures may guide us in
our '"respiritualising"' of nature (256) [lots
more examples of biologists celebrating indigenous
knowledges page 256]. Some conceptions approach
Bataille's, or chaos theory, or Gaia.
There was apparently a 'National Forum on
Biodiversity' in 1986 organised by scientists
which included indigenous peoples like Larry
Littlebird, a native American, who intrigued the
audience. Despite this openness, however, 'the
centrality of homosexuality/transgender to
indigenous belief systems' (257) was missed.
Littlebird's own tribe recognises two-spirits and
homosexual and transgender humans and animals, and
references in his story to particular animals
clearly connected to these issues.
There is a convergence between scientific and
indigenous thoughts over notions of gender and
sexual fluidity. One route from linear
thinking led to quantum theory [!], but this led
us back into 'the lap of the Great Mystery'
already central to indigenous knowledge. [No
need for all those ingenious experiments then]
There are insights to inform our current '"Crisis
of Perception"', and modern identity politics. The
'teachings of tribal people' still need to be
acknowledged, especially the 'pivotal role played
by homosexuality and transgender'. Some modern
books are keen to cite people like Native American
poets, and they also offer 'juxtaposing
references to specific constructs of Western
science such as quantum physics or molecular
structures'. One often-cited person is also a
lesbian who celebrates androgyny — although it is
not common to acknowledge these characteristics.
The poet concerned [Harjo] talks of a unifying
life force linking all things in the Great Mystery
[she actually cites a Sioux chieftain], and this
has been developed by writers on wildlife
conservation. Such people have also appreciated
the 'sophisticated game management practices'
developed by Native Americans, sometimes based on
the spiritual role played by animals like bison.
Bisons can also be homosexual, although again this
is often misrecognised [by illustrations in
Western books].
If we need indigenous perspectives, we must also
include their views on sexual variety, however
unpopular. Native people should not be
'sanitised'. Admiration for their environmental
views, 'something of a cliche' (259) should extend
to their notions of sexual diversity, and the
positive role played for it [and one example notes
another suggestion for the connections between
homosexuality and fecundity]. It might seem
paradoxical to recognise apparently
counter-productive sexual activity, but it should
be grasped as part of a more general whole, linked
to 'the vitality of the natural world'.
A certain K Schlesier has studied Cheyenne shamans
and noticed the connections with new scientific
paradigms, by recognising cosmic energy as 'a
power that controls quantum phenomena' and
explains other paradoxes and ambiguities. Central
is a gender-mixed or two-spirit shaman. with
nature as a totality that can join opposites.
Sexual ambiguity is seen as 'a manifestation of
the sacred oneness and plenitude' (260). So we are
making progress by looking both to indigenous and
futuristic sources, including rethinking things
like shape shifting and morphing when applied to
gender. The animal world is already 'brimming with
countless gender variations and shimmering sexual
possibilities'. [Another example] Monarch
butterflies gather in their hundreds of thousands,
sometimes mating both in homo and hetero forms
[homo forms apparently occupy 'more than 10% of
the Monarch pairs', (261)] They evoke biological
exuberance.
So we have been on a 'journey' threading together
a number of strands, sometimes following
'tangential meanderings' until we get to the
concept of Biological Exuberance. Now animal
sexual diversity makes sense, even though it might
be inexplicable according to 'conventional
definitions of usefulness'. Such failure
explanation has led to something new, however 'a
new way of seeing the world'. We now see the
connections between animal sexual diversity and
our everyday lives. We can see examples of
exuberance everywhere. Many of us have clearly
experienced natural variety, sometimes as
ecstatic. We can see it even in rundown
neighbourhoods. We have synthesised some
scientific views that are at the same time 'in
accordance with some of the most ancient
indigenous conceptions' (262). We have dissolved
binaries, honoured difference and irregularity
without having to manage them, and embraced
paradox and 'unspeakable inexplicability'
[retained a sense of mystery and awe], affirmed
the vitality of life and 'infinite possibilities'.
There is then a species by species 'bestiary'. I
am only going to bother with the Bonabo.
They live in mixed sex and mixed age communities
with temporary subgroups that are more fluid.
Female adolescent Bono though 'typically' (269)
leave their home group go to a new one, but males
remain in the same one for life. Females form
'strongly bonded subgroups', and are generally
dominant. The mating system is 'promiscuous' with
interchangeable hetero partners.
They have one of the most 'varied and extensive
repertoires of homosexual practices'. Females do
'mutual genital stimulation', possibly uniquely,
usually in a face-to-face embrace, more commonly
than with hetero. They rubbed their genitals
together (GG rub) 'directly stimulating each
other's clitoris' (270) [must have used a zoom
lens]. Their genitals may have been designed
particularly for lesbian interactions. They thrust
at 'a rate of about two thrusts per second', the
same as males in hetero, but sideways. They last
an average of 15 seconds compared to 12 seconds
for hetero.
They 'experience intense pleasure — and probably
orgasm' shown by their 'facial expressions,
vocalisations, and genital engorgement'. Lesbian
partners maintain eye contact. They can grin or
utter 'screams or squeals that are thought to be
associated with sexual climax'. The clitoris is
prominent and well developed and it undergoes full
erection. Is even possible to perform penetration.
There may be different positions. Females may
'often "negotiate" positions', by lying down to
see if the other wants to go on top (271). This
goes on among females of all ages, but often the
younger female will be on top. It is 'also… more
common' with different ranks. There may be
preliminary courtship signals — 'approaching the
partner, peering closely, standing on the hind
legs and raising the arms overhead while making
eye contact and/or touching the shoulder or knee
while staring'. With captive Bonobo there may be a
'highly developed "lexicon" of manual gestures' as
we saw.
Individuals may have multiple sexual partners —
for example in one troop 'each female interacted
sexually with five other females on average'.
There may be simultaneous group sexual activity.
Particularly '"attractive"' animals (272) might
get more action. Attraction refers to 'the shape,
size and coloration of the genital swellings'.
Relationships seem to be strongly bonded and
include other social activities. Same sex bonds
'form the core of social organisation'. Some older
ones may act as mentors. There is a kind of
'homosexual "incest taboo"' with relations tending
not to be chosen as partners, although there is
some homosexual activity 'between mothers and
their daughters'.
Males have a variety of interactions. They may
stimulate each other's genitals in different
positions, thrust by rubbing their erections
together. There is no anal penetration. Older ones
are more often on top. They may 'engage in what is
known as penis fencing', or rump rubbing
('mutually rubbing the anal and scrotal regions.
Both males often have erections'. There are
different sorts of mounting activities. They may
switch positions. They may 'scream or grin in
sexual arousal'. They may indulge in 'oral sex or
fellatio' especially with younger males. There may
be manual stimulation, usually with adult males as
the masturbator. There may be ' openmouthed
kisses, often with extensive mutual tongue
stimulation' (273).
Homosexual activity is 'nearly as common as
heterosexual activity', most of which is between
females. They may be brief periods of sexual
activity throughout the day, where each female
participates in rubbing 'on average once every two
hours or so'. Virtually all of them are
'bisexual', and motherhood is integrated with
homosexual activity. Homo and hetero may be
interspersed or alternated, or even occur
simultaneously in groups. 'It appears that — among
some females at least — homosexual activity is
preferred'. Some females have been seen
'consistently ignoring males… Preferring instead
to GG rub with each other'.
Hetero activity also has non-reproductive
behaviours, including 'rump rubbing, fellatio, and
manual stimulation'. There may be reversed mounts,
there may be copulation without penetration or
ejaculation. Both sexes masturbate, sometimes with
inanimate objects. It can be group sexual
activity, including homo/hetero threesomes. Hetero
activity can take place in several bouts in rapid
succession. There are frequent sexual invitations
'often associated with begging for food' and this
can even become annoying, especially with males
who will 'try to avoid further heterosexual
interactions' (273 – 4).
Females can cooperate in attacking males. They
mate during all parts of the female sexual cycle,
so a third of copulations 'occur during periods
when fertilisation is unlikely or impossible'
(274). Pregnancy does not stop mating. Adults
interact with adolescents and juveniles. Young
females can undertake homosexual mating but not
get pregnant. Infants are also involved, and
infants can initiate the activity 'about a third
of the time': we are talking about genital rubbing
and copulatory postures including penetration (of
adults mercifully). Bonobo have even been seen
'engaging in playful sexual interactions with red
tailed monkeys… In the wild'.
A massive list of references in zoological
journals ensues. Some of it refers to sexual
activity as an alternative to aggression, while
others bang on about the tension regulation
functions, in captive bonobo, that is. I have
summarised a couple only here
boy that was fun! back to
Barad page
|
|